logo
Fish believed to be sardines or pilchards wash up in the hundreds in Hobart

Fish believed to be sardines or pilchards wash up in the hundreds in Hobart

Hundreds of small, dead fish seen floating and washing ashore on the River Derwent near Hobart on Saturday have raised concerns with some local residents.
The fish, seen along the shore between Crayfish Point in Taroona and Long Beach in Sandy Bay, are believed to be pilchards or sardines.
David Boyles lives by the water in the Hobart suburb of Sandy Bay and has fished in the Derwent River for years.
Mr Boyles said on Saturday morning he noticed "little shiny, white-looking things on the rocks down below our house".
Along with the dead fish seen on the rocks, Mr Boyles said, "were hundreds if not thousands more, just floating in the water, dead".
He said he was distressed to see that about a third of the third appeared to be bleeding from their gills and vent, leading him to think it was "some kind of toxic event".
He said a man kayaking on the river towards Hobart told him there were many more down river.
The Environment Protection Authority (EPA) and Institute of Marine Antarctic Science (IMAS) have been contacted for comment.
Independent water scientist Christine Coughanowr has spent years monitoring the River Derwent.
For 20 years, she led the Derwent Estuary Program, which focuses on monitoring and restoring water quality of the estuary.
Ms Coughanow said while the incident had worried some, it was too early to draw conclusions.
"I think really that raises in some ways more questions than it answers."
She said it was still unknown if other marine species were impacted, and how widespread the issue was.
"I think really the key question is for those who have the ability to assess this, the EPA and the fish health authorities, to get the samples and do the necessary analysis to find out what's going on," Ms Coughanowr said.

Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

Clever cockatoos learn to turn on and drink from bubblers
Clever cockatoos learn to turn on and drink from bubblers

ABC News

time18 hours ago

  • ABC News

Clever cockatoos learn to turn on and drink from bubblers

First they figured out how to open bins, now sulphur-crested cockatoos in Western Sydney have taught members of their flock to use public drinking fountains. The behaviour was observed in a group of around 200 birds - the research has been published in 'The Royal Society Biology Letters.' Co-author of the study and behavioural ecologist, Dr Lucy Aplin, spoke with ABC NewsRadio's Sarah Morice and said it remained a mystery as to why the birds are opting to use the bubbler, over more easily available and accessible water sources.

Cockatoos stun by learning human-like trick
Cockatoos stun by learning human-like trick

News.com.au

time19 hours ago

  • News.com.au

Cockatoos stun by learning human-like trick

Australia's iconic bird has added an impressive skill to its repertoire of making an absolute racket and looking like an avian Liberace — as cockatoos have been observed operating water bubblers using only beaks and claws. A new study, published in The Royal Society Biology Letters, reveals that a population of Sulphur-crested cockatoos in western Sydney has adopted a new novel behaviour – manipulating twist-handle drinking fountains to get water. The study found the behaviour has been persisting for at least two years in the area and appears to be widely adopted within the local population. Researchers from institutions including the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior and Western Sydney University observed the birds intensively at parklands in Doonside for 44 days. In that time they recorded 525 attempts by cockatoos to get a drink. Operating a water fountain designed for humans is not a simple task when you are about 50cm tall, weigh less than 1.2kg and are a cockatoo. It requires a co-ordinated sequence of actions, often involving using both claws and their beak while shifting their body weight to turn the twist-handle and keep the water flowing. It's a complex manoeuvre, yet researchers observed a success rate of 41 per cent with that rate jumping to 51 per cent for the 24 birds they had temporarily colour-marked. The report said the behaviour drew 'interesting parallels' to the cockatoos famous bin-opening technique, 'where 54 per cent of attempts by marked birds were successful, suggesting similarities in either their physical difficulty or time taken to learn'. 'However, unlike the bin-opening innovation, where 32 per cent of marked individuals in the local population attempted, here an estimated 70 per cent of marked individuals attempted, with no evidence for ongoing spread,' researchers said. 'This suggests that the drinking fountain innovation had already undergone extensive social diffusion prior to the study.' Dr John Martin, a senior ecologist at Ecosure and co-author of the study, said the birds likely first learned what to do by watching people. 'Eventually one of them got it, and then the others were like, 'ah, this is fun'.' The cockatoos learned by watching others and then trying themselves,' he told The Guardian. Interestingly, the study found no evidence of a sex bias in either attempting to use the fountains or succeeding. This is a departure from the bin-opening innovation, which was heavily biased towards males. Researchers speculate this difference might be due to extrinsic factors – perhaps bin lids require more physical strength, or competition is higher at bins, favouring dominant males. Another departure from the bin-opening behaviour lies in the technique's diffusion among the cockatoo population. The drinking fountain behaviour appears to be confined to the home range of a single roost whereas the bin opening spread rapidly across many suburbs. So why the limited spread? The short answer is the design of the water fountain. Bubblers vary from fountain-to-fountain and suburb-to-suburb while bin designs remain relatively uniform across councils. The study also found that while individual cockatoos have their own idiosyncratic techniques for operating the fountain, successful sequences of actions were less variable in composition than unsuccessful ones. This indicates that while individual learning plays a role, there might be a 'correct' or more efficient way to operate the fountain that skilled birds converge upon.

Eating disorder research and funding falling behind
Eating disorder research and funding falling behind

ABC News

time21 hours ago

  • ABC News

Eating disorder research and funding falling behind

New research published today is calling for a drastic re-think of how we research and treat eating disorders with current treatments for anorexia nervosa now decades old and only effective in about half of all patients. Breakthroughs have been few and far between in recent years and the need for updated funding and treatment has never been more critical with anorexia nervosa having one of the highest premature death rates of any mental illness. Guest: Associate Professor Andrea Phillipou, Principal Research Fellow in Eating Disorders at Orygen and The Centre for Youth Mental Health at The University of Melbourne Associate Professor Andrea Phillipou, Principal Research Fellow in Eating Disorders at Orygen and The Centre for Youth Mental Health at The University of Melbourne Producer: Grace Stranger If this conversation has raised any issues for you, help is available: Butterfly Foundation National Helpline on 1800 ED HOPE (1800 33 4673) Lifeline on 13 11 14

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into the world of global news and events? Download our app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store