Bored Capuchin Monkeys Are Kidnapping Howler Babies in Weird New 'Trend'
Off the coast of Panama, on an island uninhabited by humans, a culture unlike any other has arisen.
On Jicarón Island, white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus imitator) rule, unmolested by predators. In their peace, and safety, they exhibit fascinating behaviors, wielding stone tools to facilitate their foraging in a way that may have once been thought unique to humans.
Now, the monkeys have taken their shenanigans a step further. They have been caught doing something that scientists had never seen before: kidnapping the babies of howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata coibensis) and carrying them around like some sort of bizarre accessory.
After poring and puzzling over the observations, a team led by behavioral ecologist Zoë Goldsborough of the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior concluded nothing explained the behavior as aptly as a fashion, or fad.
"Scientists continue to uncover evidence of culture across animal taxa, and a behavior is considered cultural if it spreads between individuals via social learning," Goldsborough told ScienceAlert. "The howler-carrying behavior we describe is indeed part of this capuchin group's culture."
Fads are not commonly identified in non-human animals. Examples include the peculiar intermittent salmon hats sported by orcas, and chimpanzees in Zambia wearing grass in their ears. These 'fads' are behaviors that the animals learn from each other, and serve no discernible purpose, like … planking, for example.
Goldsborough and her colleagues had set up camera traps around Jicarón to monitor the capuchins' fascinating tool use. The scientists first got wind that something strange was going on with the monkeys when one individual, named Joker, was spotted going about his business with a baby howler monkey clinging to his fur.
"It was so weird that I went straight to my advisor's office to ask him what it was," Goldsborough says in a statement. Alerted that shenanigans were indeed afoot, the researchers started paying closer attention. Goldsborough studied data collected by the camera traps, and found evidence of Joker carrying, at different times, four different baby howler monkeys.
Then, it got weirder. Several months later, the behavior re-emerged. At first, the team thought Joker was resuming his strange hobby – but then they realized different capuchins were getting involved. In total, their observations over a 15-month period found five capuchins (including Joker) carrying 11 different howler monkey babies.
At first, the puzzled scientists thought that the capuchins might be adopting the babies. But interspecies adoption is rare, and usually conducted by females. All five of the howler-carrying capuchins were males. In addition, the capuchins didn't seem interested in caring for the babies – all are assumed to have eventually died of starvation, with four confirmed dead.
"The capuchin carriers do not seem to interact with the howler infants a lot besides carrying them. So they do not play with them, or try to groom them," Goldsborough explained to ScienceAlert.
"To me, it appears less as if they want to keep the howler infant because they are so interested in them and interacting with them, but more that they carry them as an 'accessory' and are interested in the carrying behavior."
But that's only part of the equation. There's the question of why are the capuchins carrying howler babies; the other question is, why these capuchins, and only these capuchins?
"This is a fascinating question, since howler monkeys and capuchin monkeys co-occur in most of their ranges, and often interact, but never like this," Goldsborough said.
Interestingly, the answer appears to be simple boredom. The capuchin population on Jicarón has no predators, and few competitors. They live a pretty cushy, relaxed life and may be somewhat understimulated, the researchers hypothesize – a lifestyle that has been linked to innovation in humans and other animals.
"We think the conditions on Jicarón Island, specifically the lack of terrestrial predators and potentially greater amount of free time, are very conducive to the innovation and spread of behaviors," Goldsborough explained.
The researchers plan to continue investigating this behavior to see if it evolves, or other fads emerge. Goldsborough also noted that she'd like to learn more about how the howler monkeys are responding to it.
Until a handful of shit-stirring capuchins decided their babies might make dapper accoutrements, they were also living in a predator-free environment. Now, their babies lives' are being threatened.
There may also be some fascinating philosophical insights to be gleaned.
"One of the reasons our discovery elicits such interest is because it provides a mirror into ourselves. Humans often try to compare ourselves to other animals to find similarities and differences, and this is usually focused on positive qualities (e.g., language, tool use, empathy)," Goldsborough said.
"However, if you think about it, humans have many seemingly arbitrary cultural traditions that harm other species. Finding that this type of culture is not limited to humans, but might also occur in other intelligent animals living in the right conditions, is a fascinating implication of our findings."
The research has been published in Current Biology.
You can also explore the documented rise and spread of the behavior on an interactive website here.
Terrifying Video Shows Earth Cracking And Sliding During Myanmar Quake
Scientists Recreated The Ancient Chemical Reactions That May Have Sparked Life
Mystery of T. Rex's Debated North American Origins Finally Solved
Hashtags

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles
Yahoo
a day ago
- Yahoo
Giant Crude Face Appears on Sun
If you look just right, the Sun appears to be opening its mouth and laughing at us. (We can't imagine why.) As ScienceAlert reports, this colossal gash forming the mouth is what's known as "coronal hole," which occurs when the star's magnetic field opens up and allows a gust of charged solar wind to rush out, leaving the area it just left much cooler and giving it that gaping look. As for the eyes, those are smaller sunspots to the hole's north, giving this solar face a look of surprise. When looking at the Sun from Earth — which we recommend only doing with special glasses or instruments, by the way — you won't notice its wide grin unless viewing it on the ultraviolet (UV) spectrum. From that wavelength, a different picture emerges: one with a wide, open mouth that laughs at our little lives back on terra firma. A fairly regular occurrence, coronal holes like this one can cause strong geomagnetic activity and even the kind of "storms" that send out bursts of charged particles towards Earth, often resulting in beautiful auroras or communication systems failures. Space weather reports indicate that this latest coronal hole is indeed causing such a storm, though it's unclear whether any tech glitches will follow in its wake. The images we're seeing of the Sun's cheeky grin don't do justice to just how huge it is. As ScienceAlert notes, this coronal hole is as wide as five Jupiters lined up side-by-side, which would put it at about 434,000 miles across. Interestingly, this is not the first time a five-Jupiter-width geomagnetic chasm has opened up to form a creepy grin on the Sun. Almost exactly 18 months ago — not long before the so-called "solar maximum," or period when the Sun's geomagnetic activity is at its peak, in October 2024 — a slightly more devilish smile was seen on our star as it blew out its ultracharged breath towards Earth. In that instance, as in this one, the geomagnetic storm associated with the coronal hole wasn't all that strong, resulting in little more than glittering auroras and a bit of social media panic. More on the Sun: New Imaging Technique Makes the Sun Look Like a Swirling Pink Liquid
Yahoo
3 days ago
- Yahoo
This ‘Tower of Worms' Is a Squirming Superorganism
When food runs out, certain tiny roundworms, barely visible to the naked eye, crawl toward one another and build living, wriggling towers that move as one superorganism. For the first time, we've caught them doing that in nature on video. Scientists spent months pointing their digital microscope at rotting apples and pears to finally catch a glimpse of these living towers formed by Caenorhabditis roundworms in an orchard that is just downhill from the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior's location in Konstanz, Germany. 'It wasn't that hard to find. It's just the people didn't have the interest or time or funding for this kind of research,' says biologist Daniela Perez, lead author of the study. Perez and her team at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior then studied this behavior in a laboratory to learn more. To spur the towering, they placed groups of Caenorhabditis elegans in a dish without food, alongside a toothbrush bristle that could work as a scaffold. Dozens of worms quickly climbed on top of the bristle and one another to form a structure that moved in an eerily coordinated manner. The tower responded to the touch of a glass pipe by attempting to latch onto it; it stretched to bridge the gap between the bottom of the dish and its lid; and it even waved its tip around to probe the surrounding environment. The results were published Thursday in Current Biology. [Sign up for Today in Science, a free daily newsletter] Researchers had previously observed this towering in the lab but didn't know that it was an actual survival strategy in the wild. 'Discovering [this behavior] in wild populations is really important as it shows this is a part of how these animals live and not just a lab artifact,' says William Schafer, a geneticist at the University of Cambridge, who studies C. elegans and was not involved in the study. Why do the worms do this? The researchers think towering helps worms set out to find richer food sources. When resources are limited, 'it probably makes sense for microscopic organisms to cooperate for dispersing by forming something bigger,' says the study's senior author Serena Ding. The towers could allow some of their members to reach new places or to hitchhike on other organisms such as fruit flies. The bigger question is how the worms communicate within the towers. If the worms on top latch onto a fly, how do those at the bottom know to detach from where they're anchored? They could communicate chemically through pheromones and mechanically through movement patterns, Schafer suggests. Perez says her team plans to test this next. 'Every time we have a meeting, we end up with 10 new project ideas,' she says. 'There are so many directions we can take this.'
Yahoo
3 days ago
- Yahoo
These worms stack together to form living towers, new study finds
Sign up for CNN's Wonder Theory science newsletter. Explore the universe with news on fascinating discoveries, scientific advancements and more. Nature seems to offer an escape from the hustle and bustle of city life, but the world at your feet may tell another story. Even in the shade of a fruit tree, you could be surrounded by tiny skyscrapers — not made of steel or concrete, but of microscopic worms wriggling and writhing into the shape of long, vertical towers. Even though these miniature architects, called nematodes, are found all over Earth's surface, scientists in Germany recently witnessed their impressive building techniques in nature for the first time. After months of closely inspecting rotten pears and apples in local orchards, researchers from the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior and the University of Konstanz were able to spot hundreds of the 1-millimeter-long (0.04-inch) worms climbing onto one another, amassing structures up to 10 times their individual size. To learn more about the mysterious physics of the soft, slimy towers, the study team brought samples of nematodes called Caenorhabditis elegans into a lab and analyzed them. There, the scientists noticed the worms could assemble in a matter of hours, with some reaching out from the twisting mass as exploratory 'arms' sensing the environment and building accordingly. But why the worms formed the structures wasn't immediately clear. The team's findings, published Thursday in the journal Current Biology, show that even the smallest animals can prompt big questions about the evolutionary purpose of social behaviors. 'What we got was more than just some worms standing on top of each other,' said senior study author Serena Ding, a Max Planck research group leader of genes and behavior. 'It's a coordinated superorganism, acting and moving as a whole.' To find out what was motivating the nematodes' building behavior, the study team tested the worms' reactions to being poked, prodded and even visited by a fly — all while stacked in a tower formation. 'We saw that they are very reactive to the presence of a stimulus,' said the study's first author, Daniela Perez, who is a postdoctoral researcher at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior. 'They sense it, and then the tower goes towards this stimulus, attaching itself to our metal pick or a fly buzzing around.' This coordinated reaction suggests the hungry nematodes may be joining together to easily hitch a ride on larger animals such as insects that transport them to (not so) greener pastures with more rotten fruit to feast on, Perez said. 'If you think about it, an animal that is 1 millimeter long cannot just crawl all the way to the next fruit 2 meters (6.6 feet) away. It could easily die on the way there, or be eaten by a predator,' Perez explained. Nematodes are capable of hitchhiking solo too, she added, but arriving to a new area in a group may allow them to continue reproducing. The structures themselves may also serve as a mode of transport, as evidenced by how some worms formed bridges across gaps within the petri dishes to get from one surface to another, Perez noted. 'This discovery is really exciting,' said Orit Peleg, an associate professor of computer science who studies living systems at the University of Colorado Boulder's BioFrontiers Institute. 'It's both establishing the ecological function of creating a tower, and it really opens up the door to do more controlled experimentation to try to understand the perceptual world of these organisms, and how they communicate within a large group.' Peleg was not involved in the study. As the next step, Perez said her team would like to learn whether the formation of these structures is a cooperative or competitive behavior. In other words, are the towering nematodes behaving socially to help each other out, or are their towers more akin to a Black Friday sale stampede? Studying the behaviors of other self-assembling creatures could offer clues to the social norms of nematodes and help answer this question, Ding said. Ants, which assemble to form buoyant rafts to survive floodwaters, are among the few creatures known to team up like nematodes, said David Hu, a professor of mechanical engineering and biology at Georgia Tech. Hu was not involved in the study. 'Ants are incredibly sacrificial for one another, and they do not generally fight within the colony,' Hu said. 'That's because of their genetics. They all come from the same queen, so they are like siblings.' Like ants, nematodes didn't appear to display any obvious role differentiation or hierarchy within the tower structures, Perez said. Each worm from the base to the top of the structure was equally mobile and strong, indicating no competition was at play. However, the lab-cultivated worms were basically clones of one another, so it's not clear whether role differentiation occurs more often in nature, where nematode populations could have more genetic differences, she noted. Additionally, socially cooperative creatures tend to use some form of communication, Peleg said. In the case of ants, it may be their pheromone trails, while honeybees rely on their ritual dance routines and slime molds use their pulsing chemical signals. With nematodes, however, it's still not clear how they might communicate — or if they are communicating at all, Ding said. 'The next steps for (the team) are really just choosing the next questions to ask.' Notably, there has been a lot of interest in studying cooperative animal behaviors among the robotics community, Hu said. It's possible that one day, he added, information about the complex sociality of creatures like nematodes could be used to inform how technology, such as computer servers or drone systems, communicates.