logo
Wild fish can tell humans apart when they dress differently, study finds

Wild fish can tell humans apart when they dress differently, study finds

The Guardian19-02-2025

Wild fish can tell people apart – at least when they are wearing different-coloured outfits – researchers have found in a study they say could shift our relationship with the creatures.
It is known that certain domestic animals – or those that live close to humans – can tell one person from another, a skill researchers say could be tied to particular humans being more inclined to share resources with them or, conversely, pose a danger. However, such discrimination is less well known in wild animals.
Now researchers have found that wild fish can tell two people apart, apparently by what they wear.
'They are just using simple mechanisms that they use every day in their lives, and they adapt it to [recognise] humans,' said Maëlan Tomasek, first author of the research from Max Planck Institute of Animal Behaviour in Germany.
Writing in the journal Biology Letters, Tomasek and colleagues report how they carried out the study in open water in the Mediterranean Sea.
In the first stage of the study, a researcher spent 12 days training wild saddled seabream and black seabream to follow her by repeatedly offering them food and rewarding those that followed when she swam away.
The trainer was then joined by another researcher dressed in either identical diving gear or diving gear with different-coloured patches and fins. In both scenarios, the two divers swam off in different directions, before returning to the starting point and repeating the process. While both researchers carried food, the fish received a reward only if they followed the trainer.
The researchers carried out 30 trials for each outfit, and used video recordings to count the number of fish following each diver. They reported that, when the divers wore different outfits, both species of fish followed the trainer more often than the other researcher, with this preference becoming more pronounced as the trials went on.
For both species, the team found some individually identifiable fish became better at choosing to follow the trainer, again suggesting the animals were learning which diver to follow.
However, when the divers wore the same outfit, no such effect was seen for black bream, while saddled bream followed the trainer more during the middle batch of trials only. 'All in all, when we wore the same outfit, we have no evidence that they could discriminate between us any more,' said Tomasek.
Sign up to Down to Earth
The planet's most important stories. Get all the week's environment news - the good, the bad and the essential
after newsletter promotion
The team say that, as the fish had no prior experience with humans, it is likely they were harnessing existing capabilities based on visual cues to tell the divers apart. 'It shows very simple mechanisms, like pattern recognition or colour recognition can be used, and co-opted to be used, in human recognition,' said Tomasek.
Tomasek added that the study could prompt us to reconsider the way we treat fish, including whether to kill and eat them. 'It's very human to not want to care about them, but the fact that they can care about us, maybe it's time that we can care about them, too,' he said.

Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

Cockatoos start sipping from Sydney's drinking fountains after mastering series of complex moves
Cockatoos start sipping from Sydney's drinking fountains after mastering series of complex moves

The Guardian

time5 days ago

  • The Guardian

Cockatoos start sipping from Sydney's drinking fountains after mastering series of complex moves

Sulphur-crested cockatoos in Sydney have worked out how to operate drinking fountains, with footage showing the white birds gripping and turning the handle before leaning in for a sip. Researchers set up camera traps and recorded the birds taking turns at a drinking fountain in western Sydney. Scientists observed more than 500 attempts over 44 days and revealed the birds were successful about 46% of the time. 'The behaviour consists of a combination of actions involving both feet, bill and shifting body weight to start the water flow,' the researchers said in their paper. Dr John Martin, a senior ecologist at Ecosure and co-author of the study, said they stumbled on the complex behaviour at a twist-handle-operated bubbler located in a western Sydney sports field while surveying cockatoo foraging habits. 'So they would go and stand on it, and then they would have to grip the handle and actually push it forwards to activate it, and then lean over and have a drink.' Presumably the birds first learned what to do by watching people, Martin said. 'Eventually one of them got it, and then the others were like, 'ah, this is fun'.' The cockatoos learned by watching others and then trying themselves, he said. About 70% of the local population attempted the manoeuvre, according to the research, published in Biology Letters. Sign up for Guardian Australia's breaking news email Drinking from the public fountain wasn't just out of necessity, he said, given there was a creek nearby, only 500 metres away. There seemed to be an element of fun, he said. 'The flock would come in, and they'd all be foraging and drinking – having a bit of a relax and a bit of a laugh.' The 'drinking fountain innovation' – which has persisted at least two years – is the second documented urban adaptation to spread across Sydney's sulphur-crested cockatoo populations. It followed the 'bin-opening innovation', where cockatoos figured out how to use their beaks and feet to lift rubbish bin lids, a behaviour that soon spread throughout Sydney's southern suburbs. Martin, also a co-author of that study, said birds used socially learned techniques specific to their cultural group. 'Birds in one area opened a bin in a certain way, and birds in another geographic area opened a bin in a different way.' Members of the public are encouraged to submit their own observations of sulphur-crested cockatoo innovation via the Big City Birds website and app. Cockatoos, a family of parrots, are known innovators, with dextrous toes that are capable of gripping. Sign up to Breaking News Australia Get the most important news as it breaks after newsletter promotion Austrian research on Goffin's cockatoos, native to Indonesia, has observed birds dunking biscuits in water before eating them, and dipping their food in yoghurt to add flavour. The problem-solving behaviours are examples of rapid, widespread social learning that helps some species better adapt to city environments. Dr Holly Parsons, who manages the urban bird program at BirdLife Australia, said sulphur-crested cockatoos were big, loud and interactive birds and a common sight in Australian cities. 'They're smart, so they're able to exploit the urban environment to get the resources that they need, and that's one of the reasons why they're doing so well living with us.' The species was the fourth most commonly spotted bird nationally in BirdLife Australia's annual Aussie Bird Count. Parsons, who had seen cockatoos flipping bin lids, said she was not surprised they could learn to operate drinking fountains. There were even stories of pet cockatoos ending up in the wild and teaching other birds to speak, she said. 'It's just so fascinating to see how their brains are working and how they are learning from each other.'

Sulphur-crested cockatoos observed using bubblers in western Sydney
Sulphur-crested cockatoos observed using bubblers in western Sydney

The Guardian

time5 days ago

  • The Guardian

Sulphur-crested cockatoos observed using bubblers in western Sydney

A new study says a population of sulphur-crested cockatoos in western Sydney have begun drinking from twist-handled public drinking fountains. Researchers published their findings in the journal Biology Letters. 'Successful operation requires a coordinated sequence of actions, with only 41% of observed attempts ending in success … To our knowledge, this behaviour has not been observed elsewhere. Altogether, this suggests that this drinking innovation has spread to form a new urban-adapted local tradition'

Bedbugs have been bothering humans since caveman times, study finds
Bedbugs have been bothering humans since caveman times, study finds

The Independent

time30-05-2025

  • The Independent

Bedbugs have been bothering humans since caveman times, study finds

A new study suggests bedbugs may have been bothering humans for about 60,000 years, with their populations booming as people formed the first cities. The research, published in Biology Letters, examined bedbug genetics and found two lineages: one that stayed on bats and one that targeted humans. As humans built early cities like Mesopotamia around 12,000 years ago, bedbug populations thrived, making them one of the first human pests. Professor Warren Booth notes that humans likely carried bedbugs from caves when they moved out around 60,000 years ago, resulting in less genetic diversity in the human-associated lineage. Bedbug infestations saw a dramatic 35 per cent increase between 2022 and 2024, highlighting their resurgence after near-eradication due to the chemical DDT.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into the world of global news and events? Download our app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store