logo
#

Latest news with #CommunicationsPsychology

Why Some People Recall Dreams Better Than Others
Why Some People Recall Dreams Better Than Others

Medscape

time21-05-2025

  • Health
  • Medscape

Why Some People Recall Dreams Better Than Others

Dreams have captivated humanity for millennia, interpreted as divine omens in ancient cultures or as Freudian insights into unconscious desires. Modern neuroscience explores dreams as a window into consciousness because they provide a naturally occurring altered state where the brain generates complex, internally-driven experiences. However, to study dreams people need to remember them, and it's not well understood what is involved in dream recall or why some people seem to remember them better than others. A new study, published in Communications Psychology , investigated the factors associated with remembering dreams in 217 healthy people aged 19-70 years who recorded their dreams every morning for 15 days while their sleep and cognitive data were tracked by wearable devices and psychometric tests. 'Dreams represent an important model for understanding how consciousness emerges in the brain,' said Giulio Bernardi, MD, PhD, professor of psychology at IMT School for Advanced Studies Lucca in Lucca, Italy, and senior author of the study. 'We know that we forget most of our dreams, and so we wanted to understand why there is this difference between different people because these are factors that are important for us in the study of consciousness.' Sleep progresses through several stages during the night: N1 is light sleep; N2 is deeper sleep; N3 is the deepest sleep, also called slow-wave sleep; and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is most associated with dreaming. These patterns cycle throughout the night. 'Within REM sleep, we usually have more vivid dreams, more perceptual dreams, and this means that these dreams are easier to remember,' explained Valentina Elce, PhD, postdoctoral researcher in Bernardi's lab and lead author of the study. Their data indicates that people who had longer, lighter sleep tended to at least remember that they dreamed — these people may have had more REM sleep. Younger people remembered more dream details than older people. Also, participants reported less dream recall during winter than during spring, suggesting environmental or circadian influences. Additionally, people who remembered more dreams tended to be people who daydreamed as well. 'This propensity of the brain to generate spontaneous experiences goes beyond sleep and also affects mental activity during the day,' Elce explained. Interestingly, people who said they didn't remember their dreams at the beginning of the study reported that they were able to remember more by the end of the study, Elce said. This indicates that the process of intentionally trying to remember and record dreams can help people remember them. 'This study had several strong points, including the longitudinal collection and the large, diverse sample size. The amount of data gathered about each participant was also impressive, ranging from physiological measurements to psychological testing,' noted Caleb Lack, PhD, psychology professor at the University of Central Oklahoma, Edmond, Oklahoma, who was not involved in the study. A weakness of the study, he said, was that all study participants were from Italy, and there may be some cultural differences in dream recall. Based on the study's findings, dream recall seems to be a result of a combination of factors like sleep conditions, thinking about dreams in the morning, and mind wandering during the day, Lack noted. 'In other words, both individual traits and your environment play a role in whether or not you remember any dreams,' he said. 'Overall, [the results] are pretty in line with prior findings and expectations based on factors we know influence whether or not you recall having dreamed.' Why we dream is still a mystery. 'The scientific community does not agree yet about the potential biological function of dreams, and one of the possible ideas is that dreams help us to consolidate our memories…but also to elaborate the emotional content of our experiences,' Elce said. 'A huge body of work has shown that our dreams are heavily influenced by what we are thinking about and what stimuli we encounter while awake,' Lack said. But psychologists no longer believe, like Freud did, that the content of our dreams has great significance in our daily lives. 'However, it's true that the state of your mental health can impact your dream content — for instance, being highly stressed can lead to more negative emotions in your dreams, or traumatic events can cause nightmares. If that's happening, addressing those difficulties is best done through evidence-based psychotherapies,' Lack said. Lack noted that cognitive-behavioral therapy has been shown to improve quality of sleep and lower nightmares in those with anxiety disorders. But if someone rarely or never remembers dreaming, it's nothing to worry about. 'The majority of people remember few to no dreams they had the prior night, although prior research shows we probably have around 2 hours of them a night, although there is pretty wide variation in this from person to person as seen in the study,' Lack said. Elce and Bernardi hope their study will help in other research. They gave an example of a study that sought to test whether dreaming helped performance in a task. It enrolled 22 people but only four people remembered dreaming about the task, so the study couldn't draw strong conclusions. Having tools to help people remember their dreams could help in similar future studies. 'Understanding what happens to the healthy sleeping brain is something crucial,' Elce noted. This study, Lack said, 'sets the stage for further understanding into just why certain people remember more of their dreams, as well as suggesting some ways to help people remember more of their dreams, if that's something they want to do.' Next, Bernardi hopes to look at dream content and eventually to 'see how dreams change in pathological conditions to see whether maybe dreams could be used as an index, as a marker of some alterations in the brain,' he explained. He wants to know if diseases like dementia or Alzheimer's lead to changes in dreaming, which could be helpful for diagnosis.

Taking intermittent quizzes reduces achievement gaps and enhances online learning, even in highly distracting environments
Taking intermittent quizzes reduces achievement gaps and enhances online learning, even in highly distracting environments

Yahoo

time14-05-2025

  • Science
  • Yahoo

Taking intermittent quizzes reduces achievement gaps and enhances online learning, even in highly distracting environments

Inserting brief quiz questions into an online lecture can boost learning and may reduce racial achievement gaps, even when students are tuning in remotely in a distracting environment. That's a main finding of our recent research published in Communications Psychology. With co-authors Dahwi Ahn, Hymnjyot Gill and Karl Szpunar, we present evidence that adding mini-quizzes into an online lecture in science, technology, engineering or mathematics – collectively known as STEM – can boost learning, especially for Black students. In our study, we included over 700 students from two large public universities and five two-year community colleges across the U.S. and Canada. All the students watched a 20-minute video lecture on a STEM topic. Each lecture was divided into four 5-minute segments, and following each segment, the students either answered four brief quiz questions or viewed four slides reviewing the content they'd just seen. This procedure was designed to mimic two kinds of instructions: those in which students must answer in-lecture questions and those in which the instructor regularly goes over recently covered content in class. All students were tested on the lecture content both at the end of the lecture and a day later. When Black students in our study watched a lecture without intermittent quizzes, they underperformed Asian, white and Latino students by about 17%. This achievement gap was reduced to a statistically nonsignificant 3% when students answered intermittent quiz questions. We believe this is because the intermittent quizzes help students stay engaged with the lecture. To simulate the real-world environments that students face during online classes, we manipulated distractions by having some participants watch just the lecture; the rest watched the lecture with either distracting memes on the side or with TikTok videos playing next to it. Surprisingly, the TikTok videos enhanced learning for students who received review slides. They performed about 8% better on the end-of-day tests than those who were not shown any memes or videos, and similar to the students who answered intermittent quiz questions. Our data further showed that this unexpected finding occurred because the TikTok videos encouraged participants to keep watching the lecture. For educators interested in using these tactics, it is important to know that the intermittent quizzing intervention only works if students must answer the questions. This is different from asking questions in a class and waiting for a volunteer to answer. As many teachers know, most students never answer questions in class. If students' minds are wandering, the requirement of answering questions at regular intervals brings students' attention back to the lecture. This intervention is also different from just giving students breaks during which they engage in other activities, such as doodling, answering brain teaser questions or playing a video game. Online education has grown dramatically since the pandemic. Between 2004 and 2016, the percentage of college students enrolling in fully online degrees rose from 5% to 10%. But by 2022, that number nearly tripled to 27%. Relative to in-person classes, online classes are often associated with lower student engagement and higher failure and withdrawal rates. Research also finds that the racial achievement gaps documented in regular classroom learning are magnified in remote settings, likely due to unequal access to technology. Our study therefore offers a scalable, cost-effective way for schools to increase the effectiveness of online education for all students. We are now exploring how to further refine this intervention through experimental work among both university and community college students. As opposed to observational studies, in which researchers track student behaviors and are subject to confounding and extraneous influences, our randomized-controlled study allows us to ascertain the effectiveness of the in-class intervention. Our ongoing research examines the optimal timing and frequency of in-lecture quizzes. We want to ensure that very frequent quizzes will not hinder student engagement or learning. The results of this study may help provide guidance to educators for optimal implementation of in-lecture quizzes. The Research Brief is a short take on interesting academic work. This article is republished from The Conversation, a nonprofit, independent news organization bringing you facts and trustworthy analysis to help you make sense of our complex world. It was written by: Jason C.K. Chan, Iowa State University and Zohara Assadipour, Iowa State University Read more: Is online education good or bad? And is this really the right question? Making the most of K-12 digital textbooks and online educational tools Universities must prepare for a technology-enabled future Jason C.K. Chan receives funding from the USA National Science Foundation. Zohara Assadipour does not work for, consult, own shares in or receive funding from any company or organization that would benefit from this article, and has disclosed no relevant affiliations beyond their academic appointment.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into the world of global news and events? Download our app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store