logo
#

Latest news with #cockatoos

Lab Notes: What makes Sydney's cockies so clever?
Lab Notes: What makes Sydney's cockies so clever?

ABC News

time17 hours ago

  • Science
  • ABC News

Lab Notes: What makes Sydney's cockies so clever?

Belinda Smith: First, they came for our rubbish. News grab: Move over Ibis, there's a new bin chicken in town. The iconic Aussie bird's mostly been prying open bin lids. Belinda Smith: And now, they're taking our water. News grab: Sulphur crested cockatoos in Western Sydney have been observed using public drinking fountains, learning to twist the handles and drinking from the bubblers. Belinda Smith: And it's not just one or two cockies doing this. More than 100 have been spotted drinking from bubblers. So, what do these entertaining exploits tell us about cocky innovation? And even about cocky culture? Hi, I'm Belinda Smith and you're listening to Lab Notes, the show that dissects the science behind new discoveries and current events. To explain why these birds are so bloody brainy is Lucy Aplin, a cognitive ecologist at the Australian National University. Now, I've seen children struggle to turn a water fountain tap and they have hands. Cockies have claws. So, how do they manage? Lucy Aplin: What they have to do is they have to hold on to the stem of the fountain. So, imagine a fountain you might get in your local sports fields, very classic upright model. So, they're gripping onto the stem of the upright part with one foot and then they're turning the twist handle with their other foot. Now, the twist handle has a spring in it. So, it'll come back so that the water doesn't run forever. So, the birds actually have to put their weight down to hold that spring. And then they have to twist their head back to drink the water while keeping the weight over the handle. So, it's quite a complex action that actually involves every body part. Belinda Smith: It's not just the strength to turn the tap but also the flexibility to kind of manoeuvre their body so that they can then enjoy the water that's coming out of the tap. Exactly, and Lucy Aplin: the foot-mouth coordination. I always say eye-hand-eye coordination, but foot-eye coordination, foot-eye-mouth coordination in this case. So, where do these particular cockies live? So, we observed this behaviour in the Western Sydney Parklands area just near Nurragingy Reserve, which is around sort of near Doonside train station, if anyone knows Western Sydney. Belinda Smith: In 2019, purely by chance, one of Lucy's colleagues, Barbara Klump, first spotted a thirsty cocky. Lucy Aplin: She was walking across the sports field and she saw them doing this. And she then walked into the Nurragingy Reserve, which is next door, and asked the rangers about it. And they said, oh yeah, they do it around here all the time. They've been doing it for ages. So, I think they thought it was a, you know, this is just what the local cockies do. As many Australians will attest, they do lots of weird and wonderful things. But we were pretty excited, so we thought, right, we have to study this. Belinda Smith: Then, not long after that initial sighting, Lucy and her team started keeping tabs on one particularly popular drinking fountain. The Lucy Aplin: drinking fountain that was most used was the one in the sports field right next to where the sleeping trees were. So, they were using it first thing in the morning and in the afternoon. How successful are they at getting water? So, we found when we did these intensive observations at one drinking fountain, that actually only about just under 50 per cent of the attempts were successful. So, individuals could try multiple times. And sometimes the attempts were unsuccessful because they were obviously distracted by the other birds that were queuing. Maybe a more dominant bird was queuing and they were trying to keep an eye on that bird at the same time and that was influencing their success. So, they were aware that this thing could produce water and were trying and were either in the process of learning or were very successful at it already. Belinda Smith: Ah, right. And so, did they learn from watching other cockies do it? Lucy Aplin: That's what we assume. So, we don't think that every individual cocky innovated this behaviour or we would be equally likely to see cockatoos drinking from drinking fountains all over Australia. Everywhere there are cockatoos and drinking fountains of which there are many places with both. So, we think that the evidence we have is highly suggestive that actually it's spreading through social learning. So, one bird or maybe a couple of birds initially invented this behaviour, worked it out and then other birds observed them and adopted the behaviour and it spread through the local group to form a local tradition in this area. So, Belinda Smith: when this behaviour was sort of first observed by your postdoc, Barbara, was this during a particularly dry Lucy Aplin: period? No, it was actually towards the end of winter. So, it would have been in August, September because that's when we're usually doing our fieldwork. So, it wasn't particularly dry. It was something that we were a little bit surprised by for that exact reason. We thought this is a really interesting behaviour for the technical innovation part of it. It seems quite complex and it's interesting to understand how it spreads and whether it's continuing to spread. But alongside that, there's this really interesting question which is what is the adaptive benefit of it? Because we'd just come off studying another innovation which was the bin opening behaviour which we're still studying in the south of Sydney. And there, it's really obvious what the cockatoos are getting out of it. They're getting bread or pasta or pizza or all high calorie items. Delicious things. Exactly. But here, there's an artificial lake that they've made in the lovely Chinese gardens in the Nurragingy Parklands. There's a local river they could be using. There's lots of local water sources and yet they seem to be preferring to do this highly complicated behaviour where often they also have to queue for it. So, it was a bit of a mystery we also wanted to solve as to why. So, did you find out? Well, we have three working hypotheses. Unfortunately, we couldn't answer them in this study but we're hoping to get to them. So, in this study, we wanted to ask just straightforward, are they using this as a supplement to other water sources? For example, when it's really hot and dry. And we saw no evidence for that. It seemed more like they were actually preferring to use drinking fountains over those other alternative water sources. So, then we had three follow on hypotheses. Maybe the water just tastes better. And that's not, I think, out of this world because if you think about whether you would rather go down to the creek, drink slimy, silty, muddy creek water. Or whether you might prefer a nice Sydney tap water, even though some people might be rude about Sydney tap water. It's probably still preferable to muddy creek water. So, that is a possibility that they're also making that choice. The other potential explanation we thought about is whether they just feel safer drinking from drinking fountains. Because they're usually, or all of these, in this case, were in picnic areas or they're on the edge of sports fields. So, they're in open areas with really good visibility. They're off the ground. And maybe that feels much safer than going down to that waterhole where maybe the predators are lurking behind the bush. Belinda Smith: Part of me also wonders if there's some payoff to actually working for the water. It makes me think of shelling pistachios. Somehow, the pistachio tastes much better if you're the one cracking it open. And it's like that little bit of work has just made the reward so much more tasty. Lucy Aplin: Yes. That exact phenomenon you're describing does have a scientific name. You know, there's jargon for everything. Yes, great. It's called contra-freeloading. And it means that you'd rather work a little bit for your reward than get it for free. And it is something that has been observed in humans. And it's also been observed in captive parrots. Belinda Smith: Okay, so we've got bin lid flipping cockies. We've got water fountain drinking cockies. What other behaviours have been reported in cockatoos? Lucy Aplin: So one I find perhaps a little bit disturbing is that they seem to have taken a liking in some parts of Sydney to drinking discarded energy drink cans or soft drink cans. So if they find them on the ground or in the rubbish, they'll pick them up using their bill into the sort of hole, you know, the drinking hole and then tip it back to try and get the drugs. Belinda Smith: Oh, my gosh. A caffeinated cocky. Exactly. Just what you need. Lucy Aplin: That's why I find that one a little bit disturbing. I'm not sure what the effect of caffeine is on cockatoos, but I don't think it'd be good. And we have going along the same sort of high energy line, just like we have observations of noisy miners in Rainbow Larrakeet stealing sugar packets, cockatoos will also occasionally steal sugar packets. That's been reported to us. And another innovation which has been reported to us from the south of Sydney, from the Northern Territory, amazingly, from a little community right up in Arnhem Land and from a couple of sites up in far north of Sydney is what we're calling the lunch bag innovation. It's a bit hard to know how to label this one. It seems to involve cockatoos going to schools where the kids leave their bags outside the classroom, unzipping the bags, taking out the lunch boxes and then undoing the lunch boxes and running off with the sandwiches. Belinda Smith: Oh, that's so cheeky. OK, so why and how can these cockies do such amazing and often annoying things? Lucy Aplin: There's a few hypotheses that have been put forward. When we look at overall brain size across parrots, it seems like one thing that fits really well is what we call the cognitive buffer hypothesis. And it's this idea that if you live a long time, which parrots do, your environment's going to be changing around you, unless you're in a really stable environment. But parrots are often not in a really stable environment. The environment's changing and having a large brain allows them to cope with that change that they might experience during their life. So it provides a buffer to the sort of swings and arrows of fortune by allowing you to work out solutions to changes. And that sort of explanation for the evolution of intelligence in parrots, it's not the only one, but that hypothesis fits really nicely with the urban adaptability that we see because urban environments are that. They're changing, novel environments where things can change. They can be pretty stable, but then they can change really rapidly when the local government decides to change all of the drinking fountains or whatever. So having a big brain might allow you to cope with that by learning new solutions and adjusting your behaviour. Belinda Smith: Wow. Just like us humans then. And cockies don't just have big brains. They also have a lot of cells or neurons in those brains. Lucy Aplin: So a cockatoo has about the same total number of neurons. So just straight count, not accounting for differences in body size or anything, just straight count. The same total number of neurons or similar to a macaque monkey, which is a medium sized monkey. Wow. So they're really punching above their weight. Like literally? Belinda Smith: Yes. These behaviours, they're kind of quirky and fun and maybe a bit messy, but they're very interesting. But what does this tell us about cocky culture? Lucy Aplin: Well, it tells us that they're very capable of learning from each other, from social learning. We have no evidence for teaching, so they're not doing that. Watch out if they start. No evidence for that. But they are very capable of observing other individuals and adopting the behaviour if it's beneficial. And they're capable of transmitting that behaviour over quite large geographic areas. Some of the studies we're doing now, they're not published yet, so I don't have the full set of results. But our preliminary results from experimental work is showing that actually new beneficial behaviours can spread over, say, the entire city of Canberra within just a couple of weeks. So the way their society is organised is really promotes that rapid spread and population level adoption of new behaviours. So I think that tells us something about the secret to their success and something about their life history in general. These are the elephants of the bird world. They're really long-lived, they're really brainy, and they're really social. And so they have a society that we recognise and we can also understand. We can look at what they're doing and understand it better than with some other species. Belinda Smith: That was Lucy Aplin, a cognitive ecologist at the Australian National University. She and her colleagues published their study about the water fountain drinking cockies in the Royal Society Biology Letters last week. And thanks for listening to Lab Notes on ABC Radio National, where every week we dissect the science behind new discoveries and current events. I'm Belinda Smith. This episode was produced on the lands of the Wurundjeri and Menang Noongar people. Fiona Pepper's the producer and it was mixed by Angie Grant. We'll catch you next week.

These clever birds can open trash cans and drink from water fountains
These clever birds can open trash cans and drink from water fountains

CNN

time6 days ago

  • Science
  • CNN

These clever birds can open trash cans and drink from water fountains

First, these parrots learned to open trash cans to forage for food. Now, they've taken it a step further – and have figured out how to turn on water fountains for a sip along with their meal. These are Australia's iconic sulphur-crested cockatoos – white birds with a yellow tuft on their heads, known for their loud, grating screech. But they're also incredibly intelligent, with large brains and nimble feet that have allowed them to pick up new habits in urban environments. The cockatoos in western Sydney, in particular, caught scientists' attention with their latest trick of drinking from public fountains. After researchers first noticed this phenomenon in 2018, they tagged 24 birds and set up cameras near fountains in the area – then sat back and watched. Throughout two months in the fall of 2019, they recorded most of the tagged birds attempting to drink from the fountains. Also known as bubblers, these fountains are operated by a twist handle – easy enough for a person to operate, but complex for an animal to figure out. Yet, the cockatoos did. They used different techniques: some would stand with both feet on the handle, while others would put one foot on the handle and one foot on the rubber spout. Then, they'd lower their body weight to turn the handle clockwise – holding the handle in place while twisting their head to take a drink. They weren't always successful – it worked about half the time, and five of the 10 drinking fountains in the area had 'chew marks' indicating cockatoos had been there before. But the success rate also meant that the cockatoos had likely been doing this for some time, said the researchers in their study, published Wednesday in the journal Biology Letters. The team had studied Sydney's sulphur-crested cockatoos before; in 2021, they published another paper examining the birds' newly observed ability to lift closed trash bin lids with their beaks and feet to access the food inside. These innovative behaviors aren't just animals being amusing or clever – they show the birds' ability to adapt to urban environments, and the power of social learning among animals, the researchers said. 'We know parrots like cockatoos are amongst the smartest birds out there, and the fact they're so social – so they have the opportunity to learn from one another if a new behavior does pop up, if one genius bird does invent something,' said one of the study's co-authors, Lucy M. Aplin, an associate professor at the Australian National University, in an interview with CNN affiliate ABC Radio. There are some questions still unanswered. The researchers don't know why exactly the cockatoos are flocking to drinking fountains, instead of other easily accessible natural water sources in the area. At first they thought the fountains might be a backup option on especially hot days when local creeks run dry – but that wasn't the case. Other theories are that the birds feel safer drinking from fountains in public areas where there are fewer predators, or that they simply prefer the taste of fountain water – but that would need further study to determine. Now, the researchers want to know what else cockatoos can do – and any habits they may have already developed that just haven't been studied yet. 'We've had some really interesting innovations reported to us, and some examples include unzipping school backpacks and stealing school lunches,' Aplin told ABC Radio. 'It has become such a problem in some areas that they have to bring the school bags into the classroom rather than leaving them outside!'

These clever birds can open trash cans and drink from water fountains
These clever birds can open trash cans and drink from water fountains

CNN

time6 days ago

  • Science
  • CNN

These clever birds can open trash cans and drink from water fountains

First, these parrots learned to open trash cans to forage for food. Now, they've taken it a step further – and have figured out how to turn on water fountains for a sip along with their meal. These are Australia's iconic sulphur-crested cockatoos – white birds with a yellow tuft on their heads, known for their loud, grating screech. But they're also incredibly intelligent, with large brains and nimble feet that have allowed them to pick up new habits in urban environments. The cockatoos in western Sydney, in particular, caught scientists' attention with their latest trick of drinking from public fountains. After researchers first noticed this phenomenon in 2018, they tagged 24 birds and set up cameras near fountains in the area – then sat back and watched. Throughout two months in the fall of 2019, they recorded most of the tagged birds attempting to drink from the fountains. Also known as bubblers, these fountains are operated by a twist handle – easy enough for a person to operate, but complex for an animal to figure out. Yet, the cockatoos did. They used different techniques: some would stand with both feet on the handle, while others would put one foot on the handle and one foot on the rubber spout. Then, they'd lower their body weight to turn the handle clockwise – holding the handle in place while twisting their head to take a drink. They weren't always successful – it worked about half the time, and five of the 10 drinking fountains in the area had 'chew marks' indicating cockatoos had been there before. But the success rate also meant that the cockatoos had likely been doing this for some time, said the researchers in their study, published Wednesday in the journal Biology Letters. The team had studied Sydney's sulphur-crested cockatoos before; in 2021, they published another paper examining the birds' newly observed ability to lift closed trash bin lids with their beaks and feet to access the food inside. These innovative behaviors aren't just animals being amusing or clever – they show the birds' ability to adapt to urban environments, and the power of social learning among animals, the researchers said. 'We know parrots like cockatoos are amongst the smartest birds out there, and the fact they're so social – so they have the opportunity to learn from one another if a new behavior does pop up, if one genius bird does invent something,' said one of the study's co-authors, Lucy M. Aplin, an associate professor at the Australian National University, in an interview with CNN affiliate ABC Radio. There are some questions still unanswered. The researchers don't know why exactly the cockatoos are flocking to drinking fountains, instead of other easily accessible natural water sources in the area. At first they thought the fountains might be a backup option on especially hot days when local creeks run dry – but that wasn't the case. Other theories are that the birds feel safer drinking from fountains in public areas where there are fewer predators, or that they simply prefer the taste of fountain water – but that would need further study to determine. Now, the researchers want to know what else cockatoos can do – and any habits they may have already developed that just haven't been studied yet. 'We've had some really interesting innovations reported to us, and some examples include unzipping school backpacks and stealing school lunches,' Aplin told ABC Radio. 'It has become such a problem in some areas that they have to bring the school bags into the classroom rather than leaving them outside!'

Clever cockatoos learn to turn on and drink from bubblers
Clever cockatoos learn to turn on and drink from bubblers

ABC News

time6 days ago

  • Science
  • ABC News

Clever cockatoos learn to turn on and drink from bubblers

First they figured out how to open bins, now sulphur-crested cockatoos in Western Sydney have taught members of their flock to use public drinking fountains. The behaviour was observed in a group of around 200 birds - the research has been published in 'The Royal Society Biology Letters.' Co-author of the study and behavioural ecologist, Dr Lucy Aplin, spoke with ABC NewsRadio's Sarah Morice and said it remained a mystery as to why the birds are opting to use the bubbler, over more easily available and accessible water sources.

Cockatoos stun by learning human-like trick
Cockatoos stun by learning human-like trick

News.com.au

time6 days ago

  • Science
  • News.com.au

Cockatoos stun by learning human-like trick

Australia's iconic bird has added an impressive skill to its repertoire of making an absolute racket and looking like an avian Liberace — as cockatoos have been observed operating water bubblers using only beaks and claws. A new study, published in The Royal Society Biology Letters, reveals that a population of Sulphur-crested cockatoos in western Sydney has adopted a new novel behaviour – manipulating twist-handle drinking fountains to get water. The study found the behaviour has been persisting for at least two years in the area and appears to be widely adopted within the local population. Researchers from institutions including the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior and Western Sydney University observed the birds intensively at parklands in Doonside for 44 days. In that time they recorded 525 attempts by cockatoos to get a drink. Operating a water fountain designed for humans is not a simple task when you are about 50cm tall, weigh less than 1.2kg and are a cockatoo. It requires a co-ordinated sequence of actions, often involving using both claws and their beak while shifting their body weight to turn the twist-handle and keep the water flowing. It's a complex manoeuvre, yet researchers observed a success rate of 41 per cent with that rate jumping to 51 per cent for the 24 birds they had temporarily colour-marked. The report said the behaviour drew 'interesting parallels' to the cockatoos famous bin-opening technique, 'where 54 per cent of attempts by marked birds were successful, suggesting similarities in either their physical difficulty or time taken to learn'. 'However, unlike the bin-opening innovation, where 32 per cent of marked individuals in the local population attempted, here an estimated 70 per cent of marked individuals attempted, with no evidence for ongoing spread,' researchers said. 'This suggests that the drinking fountain innovation had already undergone extensive social diffusion prior to the study.' Dr John Martin, a senior ecologist at Ecosure and co-author of the study, said the birds likely first learned what to do by watching people. 'Eventually one of them got it, and then the others were like, 'ah, this is fun'.' The cockatoos learned by watching others and then trying themselves,' he told The Guardian. Interestingly, the study found no evidence of a sex bias in either attempting to use the fountains or succeeding. This is a departure from the bin-opening innovation, which was heavily biased towards males. Researchers speculate this difference might be due to extrinsic factors – perhaps bin lids require more physical strength, or competition is higher at bins, favouring dominant males. Another departure from the bin-opening behaviour lies in the technique's diffusion among the cockatoo population. The drinking fountain behaviour appears to be confined to the home range of a single roost whereas the bin opening spread rapidly across many suburbs. So why the limited spread? The short answer is the design of the water fountain. Bubblers vary from fountain-to-fountain and suburb-to-suburb while bin designs remain relatively uniform across councils. The study also found that while individual cockatoos have their own idiosyncratic techniques for operating the fountain, successful sequences of actions were less variable in composition than unsuccessful ones. This indicates that while individual learning plays a role, there might be a 'correct' or more efficient way to operate the fountain that skilled birds converge upon.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into the world of global news and events? Download our app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store