logo
How Vancouver's urban orcas create connections and community among followers

How Vancouver's urban orcas create connections and community among followers

Yahoo18-07-2025
VANCOUVER — Chatchawan Jaksuwong says he used to feel empty when he looked at the ocean.
But after encounters with whales in the urban waters of Vancouver the photographer now feels a deep sense of connection, and he's found community with fellow enthusiasts.
"It's a switch from that feeling that I used to have before to, I don't know, let's say (a) warmer feeling," says Jaksuwong, who moved to the city from Thailand about two years ago.
He grows emotional as he describes the new-found sense of expectation when he gazes at the ocean.
"You know there's something there that makes you feel OK."
Jaksuwong is among a growing community of whale fans who track and share the surging number of sightings around Vancouver.
Experts say the return of orcas, humpbacks and other marine mammals has been decades in the making, following the end of commercial whaling in British Columbia in the late 1960s along with the wind-down of the large-scale culling of seals and sea lions, the primary food for certain killer whales.
Andrew Trites, who leads the Marine Mammal Research Unit at the University of B.C., says whales are the "ambassadors of the Salish Sea," and their recovery is an opportunity to boost public awareness and encourage protections for their habitat.
"People care about what they see, and unfortunately, they see very little below the water's surface to understand the richness of life and the need to maintain a healthy ocean," says Trites, a professor in the school's zoology department.
Jaksuwong once saw a whale from a distance during a tour off Vancouver Island. But he says he never expected to see whales from shore in the city.
"Now I'm obsessed with orcas," he says, laughing. "It's my thing now."
In one encounter last month, he raced to catch up with a pod of whales reported to be passing Stanley Park. He caught a bus, then ran to the middle of Lions Gate Bridge that overlooks the city's Burrard Inlet in hopes of an overhead shot of the whales using his telephoto lens.
"I've never run that fast before (with) the gear and stuff, right?" he recalled in an interview. "If it's meant to be, it's meant to be. That's what I thought."
He was greeted with scenes worthy of a wildlife documentary — the whales were hunting a seal. Several of his photos show blood in the water roiling around the orcas.
"I've never seen this ever in my life. I feel so lucky, to be honest, to see that here, in nature," he says.
Jaksuwong was "over the moon" to capture a photo of a young orca leaping out of the water, a moment he had been waiting for, he says.
He shared the photos with the Facebook group "Howe Sound and Sea to Sky wildlife sightings," devoted to tracking and sharing encounters with orcas and other wildlife in the region. It has almost 27,000 members.
That day, June 14, the orcas had a bigger audience than usual — a crowd was gathered at Locarno Beach to watch a triathlon, with the whales stealing attention from the finish of the men's event.
Jaksuwong joined the Facebook group in May, when a grey whale known as Little Patch spent several weeks feeding in Vancouver's waters.
Since then, he's become friends with fellow enthusiasts.
"We share our interests and we kind of like text each other, 'whale here, whale there,' and then we go see them together," he says.
Erin Gless, executive director of the Pacific Whale Watch Association, which represents 30 companies in both B.C. and Washington state, says there has been an "exponential increase" in sightings around Vancouver in recent years.
It has given whale-watching operators the opportunity to share stories about the whales as individuals, fostering a sense of personal connection, she says.
"We're going to tell you that this humpback is nicknamed Malachite, and he was born in 2021, and he goes to Mexico in the winter," Gless says.
"That's what we're really trying to do is put a much more personalized spin on these animals, so that they're not anonymous."
Trites says he came to B.C. around 1980 and "never thought" he'd see a humpback in local waters in his lifetime, after whaling decimated the population.
"It took the humpback whales a century to find their way back here again," he says.
There were no other marine mammals to be seen regularly either at Vancouver's Spanish Banks beach or the Stanley Park seawall, he says, after the culling of seals and sea lions in the name of safeguarding fisheries.
The end of the cull around 1970 laid the groundwork for the long-term recovery of Bigg's killer whales, also known as transient orcas, which hunt mammals.
"So, we go basically from looking at what I would say was a relatively empty ocean in terms of marine mammals to one now that literally any day I can go and find a whale or a seal or a sea lion or a dolphin or a porpoise," Trites says.
The seal population has been stable for some years, kept in check by the orcas; but they have spread out to areas where people are more likely to see them, he adds.
"To me, it's a sign that if people just got out of the way, then Mother Nature can heal itself," he says.
There is an exception, however, in the story of recovery, Trites says.
Bigg's orcas differ from the southern resident killer whales that frequent the Salish Sea in the summer months. That population is endangered and at risk of extinction due in large part to declining numbers of their preferred prey, chinook salmon.
In Vancouver's busy waters, whales are also at risk of ship strikes, Trites says, while noise from vessels disrupts their ability to feed and communicate.
Gless says people are lucky to be part of the story of the whales' return, but "we need to keep it that way."
"We can't be like, 'Oh, they're recovered enough, so now let's go ahead and build this new pipeline or increase shipping traffic.' Those are all things that still concern us."
Jaksuwong, meanwhile, continues to watch for whales, monitoring sightings and making his way to the seawall as often as he can, alerting others along the way.
"You see the look on their face when they see the orcas," he says. "It's rewarding for me too."
This report by The Canadian Press was first published July 18, 2025.
Brenna Owen, The Canadian Press
Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

Why does your mind goes 'blank'? New brain scans reveal the surprising answer
Why does your mind goes 'blank'? New brain scans reveal the surprising answer

Yahoo

time4 hours ago

  • Yahoo

Why does your mind goes 'blank'? New brain scans reveal the surprising answer

When you buy through links on our articles, Future and its syndication partners may earn a commission. You look up from your phone screen and suddenly realize you weren't thinking about anything. It's not a lapse in memory or a daydream; it's literally a moment when you're not thinking of anything at all. Neuroscientists have a term for it — mind blanking — which they define as a brief, waking state when conscious thought simply stops. Scientists used to think our waking minds were always generating thoughts, but recent research shows that's not the case. Mind blanking is now recognized as a distinct conscious state associated with changes in arousal, which in neuroscience refers to alertness and responsiveness to stimuli. Studying this curious state could shed light on how consciousness works, some researchers think. "For some, it's kind of a blip in the mind, and suddenly there's nothing," Thomas Andrillon, a neuroscience researcher at the French National Institute of Health and Medical Research and the Paris Brain Institute, told Live Science. "But not with that feeling, 'There was something that I forgot.'" Often, people are unaware of the lapse until they are prompted to answer "What were you just thinking about?" "When we interrupt them randomly," Andrillon continued, "it's clear it's more frequent than what people realize." Although the frequency of this phenomenon varies among individuals, various studies suggest about 5% to 20% of a person's waking hours may be spent in this state. Related: Super-detailed map of brain cells that keep us awake could improve our understanding of consciousness An investigation of 'mind blanking' In a study published in the July issue of the journal Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Andrillon and his team used electroencephalography (EEG) — which involves placing electrodes on participants' heads — to measure brain activity while people experienced lapses in attention, such as mind wandering or mind blanking. Mind wandering occurs when people's thoughts drift to tasks or ideas unrelated to the one at hand, while mind blanking involves the absence of all thought. While wearing EEG caps, participants watched numbers flash rapidly on a display screen. They were instructed to press a button every time a number appeared except for 3, which they were told to skip. This task tests how quickly people react when a response is required and how well they can inhibit that response, when necessary. Because most of the presented numbers required a response, people often pressed the button by mistake when they saw a 3 onscreen. The researchers paused the task once a minute to ask what the participants were thinking, finding that they were either focused on the task, their mind was wandering, or they were experiencing a "mind blank." Participants pressed the button more quickly when their minds were wandering, whereas their responses slowed noticeably during mind blanking, suggesting these two mental states are distinct. Brain activity told a similar story. The EEG data showed that the participants' brain activity tended to slow down slightly more when their minds were blank than when they were wandering, compared to the baseline of their paying attention. 'The connectivity changes as if the inner workings of the brain were specific, in a way, to that state," Andrillion said. EEG data is great for tracking rapid changes in brain activity, but it can't pinpoint exactly which brain regions are involved. That's in part because it records brain waves through the skull, and the signals blur as they make their way through the brain tissue, fluid and bone. Andrillon explained it's like listening through a wall. You can tell if a group inside is noisy or quiet, but you can't tell who is talking. The EEG results from the study suggest that during mind blanking, the brain's activity slows down globally, but the technique couldn't identify specific areas. That's where functional MRI (fMRI) came in. Related: 'Hyper-synchronized' brain waves may explain why different psychedelics have similar effects, rat study reveals Hypersynchronization fMRI provides a clearer view of which regions are active and how they interact, but its tracking speed is slower because the technique tracks bloodflow, rather than directly following brain signals. fMRI is more like peeking into the room and seeing who's talking to whom, but not knowing precisely when, Andrillion said. Study co-author Athena Demertzi, a neuroscience researcher at the GIGA Institute-CRC Human Imaging Center at the University of Liège in Belgium, led the fMRI portion of the study. As people rested in an fMRI scanner with no particular task at hand, Demertzi and her team periodically asked what they were thinking. The results were surprising: when people reported mind blanking, their brains showed hyperconnectivity — a global, synchronized activity pattern similar to that seen in deep sleep. Typically, when we are awake and conscious, our brain regions are connected and communicating but not synchronized, as they appear to be during mind blanks. "What we think happens in the case of mind blanking is that the brain is pushed a little bit toward the side of synchronization," Andrillon said. "That might be enough to disrupt these sweet spots of consciousness, sending our mind to blank." Research into mind blanking is still in its early stages, but Andrillon and Demertzi noted that its similarity to brain patterns seen during deep sleep may offer an important clue as to its function. Deep sleep, also known as slow-wave sleep, coincides with important cleanup work for the brain. It clears away accumulated waste, cools the brain, conserves energy and helps reset the system after a full day of mental activity. RELATED STORIES —Why do we forget things we were just thinking about? —Electronic' scalp tattoos could be next big thing in brain monitoring —'Hyper-synchronized' brain waves may explain why different psychedelics have similar effects, rat study reveals Andrillon and Demertzi suggested mind blanking may act as a mini-reset while we're awake. Demertzi said it's like "taking five to steam off" or "to cool your head." Early studies in Demertzi's lab suggest sleep-deprived people report more mind blanks, adding support to this idea. Both researchers stressed that this state is likely a way for the brain to maintain itself, though "it's not ideal for performance," Andrillon said. Andrillon believes it's possible but unlikely that there are people who have never experienced mind blanking. Detecting a mind blank can be a challenge. "It can require being interrupted," Andrillon said, "to realize, 'OK, actually, there was no content.'"

Rare fossa cubs born in Paris zoo as wild numbers shrink in Madagascar
Rare fossa cubs born in Paris zoo as wild numbers shrink in Madagascar

Yahoo

time7 hours ago

  • Yahoo

Rare fossa cubs born in Paris zoo as wild numbers shrink in Madagascar

Three fossa cubs have been born at a Paris zoo – a rare event for one of Madagascar's most elusive and endangered mammals. The births offer a small but important boost to conservation efforts for a species that few people have ever seen, and which is disappearing fast in the wild. The baby fossas were first shown to the public this month at the Paris Zoological Park in Vincennes. Many visitors will have never heard of the species, which looks a little like a small wild cat but is more closely related to mongooses. 'It looks like a mini tree-dwelling puma, with a tail that's about the same length as its body – maybe even a bit longer,' said zoo veterinarian Alexis Lécu. Even in Madagascar, the fossa is hard to spot. Its forest habitat is shrinking every year due to farming and logging. Scientists believe that only around 3,000 fossas remain in the wild. 'Our visitors are often discovering this species for the first time. Even when you go to Madagascar, you see more lemurs than fossas,' Lécu said. Wild chimpanzees beat the bush telegraph using tree-root rhythms Elusive and under pressure Fossas are difficult to study. 'They only comes from one island and are not a species that's spread out over a continent. Their whole system of hunting and lifestyle is based on hiding,' Lécu explained. The species plays a key role in Madagascar's ecosystems, but is classed as vulnerable as human activity puts it at risk. The cubs, born in May, are among just 10 fossas born in zoos around the world this year. Their arrival in Paris is seen as a success for global breeding efforts. Staff at the zoo closely monitor the fossas' behaviour, rest and reproduction, sharing their findings with partners in Madagascar. They are also working with local conservationists to protect the species in the wild. That includes helping people who depend on the island's forests for farming, firewood and food. 'The aim is to help local populations live – to find ways of making a living that work, while continuing to protect species like lemurs, fossas and reptiles,' Lécu said. Zimbabwe's elephant boom fuels conflict alongside conservation wins No return to the wild The three cubs will eventually be sent to other zoos around the world as part of a breeding programme to help maintain a healthy global population. But for now, there is no plan to reintroduce them into the wild. Zoos like Vincennes are one of the few safe spaces left for fossas – and a reminder of what could be lost if Madagascar's forests continue to disappear.

Dolphins in the Potomac River? There are a lot more than you think.
Dolphins in the Potomac River? There are a lot more than you think.

Washington Post

time8 hours ago

  • Washington Post

Dolphins in the Potomac River? There are a lot more than you think.

REEDVILLE, Va. — Aboard a small motorboat, the trio of researchers navigated the waters of Chesapeake Bay. It was two hours into their trip, and there was no sign of what they'd come looking for. They watched and waited, at one point plucking a plastic bottle from the water. The researchers, based out of Georgetown University, were accustomed to long stretches with nothing but birds and fishing boats as far as the eye could see.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into a world of global content with local flavor? Download Daily8 app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store