
Scientists make surprising discovery about what lies beneath the Antarctica Ice Sheet
But scientists are finally uncovering what Antarctica's landscape really looks like – and it's not what they expected.
It's well known that much of the surface underneath the smooth ice sheet consists of mountains, valleys and deep troughs.
Now, a new study using radio-echo sounding reveals there are vast areas of 'remarkably' flat surfaces along the 3,500km stretch of East Antarctic coastline.
These previously unmapped surfaces were once connected and it is believed were formed by large rivers after East Antarctica and Australia broke apart around 80 million years ago.
The flat areas – now hidden beneath the ice sheet – are separated by deep trenches which fast-moving glaciers are steered through.
Lead author Dr Guy Paxman, from Durham University, said: 'The landscape hidden beneath the East Antarctic Ice Sheet is one of the most mysterious not just on Earth, but on any terrestrial planet in the solar system.
'When we were examining the radar images of the sub-ice topography in this region, these remarkably flat surfaces started to pop out almost everywhere we looked.'
The study, published in the journal Nature Geoscience, also revealed the ice above these flat surfaces is moving much more slowly.
Ice loss from Antarctica is increasing – boosted by global warming – but the flat areas appear to act as a barrier to ice flow and may currently be regulating the rate of ice loss, the team said.
This finding is significant, as East Antarctica has the potential to raise global sea levels by 52 metres if it were to melt completely.
Adding the flat surface effects into models of future ice sheet behaviour could help refine projections of how the ice sheet might react to climate change, the researchers said.
'The flat surfaces we have found have managed to survive relatively intact for over 30 million years, indicating that parts of the ice sheet have preserved rather than eroded the landscape,' Dr Paxman said.
'Information such as the shape and geology of the newly mapped surfaces will help improve our understanding of how ice flows at the edge of East Antarctica.
'This in turn will help make it easier to predict how the East Antarctic Ice Sheet could affect sea levels under different levels of climate warming in the future.'
The extensive flat surfaces were found beneath approximately 40 per cent of the East Antarctic Ice Sheet's 3,500km-long coastline between Princess Elizabeth Land and George V Land.
The preservation of these enigmatic surfaces over tens of millions of years indicates a lack of intense, selective erosion of these areas throughout Antarctica's glacial history.
Research co-author Professor Neil Ross, from Newcastle University, said: 'We've long been intrigued and puzzled about fragments of evidence for 'flat' landscapes beneath the Antarctic ice sheets.
'This study brings the jigsaw pieces of data together, to reveal the big picture: how these ancient surfaces formed, their role in determining the present-day flow of the ice, and their possible influence on how the East Antarctic Ice Sheet will evolve in a warming world.'
The team emphasised the need to further explore the influence of these flat surfaces on ice sheet movement during past warmer climates.
This would include drilling through the ice to retrieve rock from the flat surfaces to understand when they were last free from ice cover.
This will help improve predictions for how the ice around this large section of the East Antarctic margin will respond as the climate and ocean warms.
A separate team of researchers recently revealed there are mysterious radio signals emerging from deep within Antarctica's ice.
The strange radio pulses were detected by the Antarctic Impulsive Transient Antenna (ANITA), an array of instruments designed to detect elusive particles called neutrinos.
Rather than detecting these cosmic particles, the researchers were baffled to find signals emerging from the ice at seemingly impossible angles.
Worryingly, they have no idea what could be causing them.
In a paper, published in Physical Review Letters, an international team of researchers explained that these findings cannot be explained by the current understanding of particle physics.
This might mean there are entirely new forms of particles and interactions at play or that these unusual signals are the product of mysterious dark matter.
Dr Stephanie Wissel, an astrophysicist from The Pennsylvania State University who worked on the ANITA team, says: 'The radio waves that we detected were at really steep angles, like 30 degrees below the surface of the ice.
'It's an interesting problem because we still don't actually have an explanation for what those anomalies are.'
Antarctica's ice sheets contain 70% of world's fresh water - and sea levels would rise by 180ft if it melts
Antarctica holds a huge amount of water.
The three ice sheets that cover the continent contain around 70 per cent of our planet's fresh water - and these are all to warming air and oceans.
If all the ice sheets were to melt due to global warming, Antarctica would raise global sea levels by at least 183ft (56m).
Given their size, even small losses in the ice sheets could have global consequences.
In addition to rising sea levels, meltwater would slow down the world's ocean circulation, while changing wind belts may affect the climate in the southern hemisphere.
In February 2018, Nasa revealed El Niño events cause the Antarctic ice shelf to melt by up to ten inches (25 centimetres) every year.
El Niño and La Niña are separate events that alter the water temperature of the Pacific ocean.
The ocean periodically oscillates between warmer than average during El Niños and cooler than average during La Niñas.
Using Nasa satellite imaging, researchers found that the oceanic phenomena cause Antarctic ice shelves to melt while also increasing snowfall.
In March 2018, it was revealed that more of a giant France-sized glacier in Antarctica is floating on the ocean than previously thought.
This has raised fears it could melt faster as the climate warms and have a dramatic impact on rising sea-levels.
Hashtags

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles


Daily Mail
4 hours ago
- Daily Mail
Birds love to boogie! Cockatoos have 30 distinct dance moves - and even link them together in elaborate routines, study finds
If you thought head banging and body rolls were limited to rock concerts or hip-hop clubs, you'd be mistaken. Experts have discovered that cockatoos have at least 30 different dance moves in their repertoire – including these two signature steps. The routines – of which 17 are newly identified – can even be performed without music. And it shows that the birds' dancing skills are far more common, complex and varied than previously thought. Researchers from Charles Sturt University in Australia and Bristol University in the UK analysed 45 videos posted on social media that showed captive cockatoos dancing. Some of the newly-documented moves include the semi-circle high, the downward head/foot sync, the fluff and the jump turn. The researchers found that some birds also performed their own individual dance moves, often by combining several of the movements in unique ways. The most common move, performed by 50 per cent of birds, was the 'downward' which involved the head bobbing in a downward motion while keeping the eyes facing forward. Next came the 'sidestep', carried out by 43.5 per cent of cockatoos. Other popular moves included the 'fluff' which involves puffing up their feathers, and the headbang. Overall, movements involving just the head were more common while the least common movements were those involving just wings. Next, the researchers investigated dancing behaviour in six cockatoos from three different species housed at Wagga Wagga Zoo in Australia. They played the birds music, an audio podcast, or no audio, and found that all birds performed dance moves, regardless of whether music was being played or not. The study, published in the journal Plos One, revealed that dancing behaviour is present in nearly half of all species of cockatoo. The authors say that further research is needed to determine whether the birds enjoy dancing and whether encouraging this behaviour could improve welfare for captive cockatoos. Professor Rafael Freire said: 'The similarities with human dancing make it hard to argue against well-developed cognitive and emotional processes in parrots, and playing music to parrots may improve their welfare. 'Further research would be beneficial to determine if music can trigger dance in captive birds and serve as a form of environmental enrichment.' Researcher Natasha Lubke added: 'As well as supporting the presence of positive emotions in birds and advancing dance behaviour as an excellent model to study parrot emotions, the work suggests that playing music to parrots may provide a useful approach to enrich their lives in captivity, with positive effects on their welfare.' Several years ago, a viral cockatoo named Snowball was enlisted to help scientists study dancing in animals after becoming a YouTube sensation. The talented bird managed to wow researchers with 14 different moves as he bopped along to 80s classics. The sulphur-crested cockatoo was filmed swinging from side to side, lunging and lifting his foot as he grooved to Another One Bites The Dust and Girls Just Want To Have Fun. Snowball showed a more diverse range of motions when bopping along to Cyndi Lauper's 1983 classic, however. Scientists led by a team from the University of California, San Diego believe his 'remarkably diverse spontaneous movements' show that dancing is not limited to humans but a response to music when certain conditions are present in the brain. British scientists have taught a group of seals how to copy human sounds and one of them how to sing the popular lullaby Twinkle, Twinkle Little Star. Researchers from the University of St Andrews worked with three young grey seals from birth to determine their natural repertoire and say the research could help others study speech disorders. The seals were then trained to copy new sounds, such as vowels and melodies, by changing their formants, the parts of human speech sounds that encode most of the information that we convey to each other. One seal, named Zola, was 'particularly good' at the musical side of things - correctly copying up to 10 notes of songs, including the classic jingle of 'Twinkle, Twinkle'. The study, published in the journal Current Biology, found that the seals used their vocal tract in the same way as us - unlike our closest relatives in the animal world, monkeys and apes. Now scientists working on disorders can use seals as a new model system to study the 'nature vs. nurture' element of our speech development. Researchers Dr Amanda Stansbury and Professor Vincent Janik, of the Scottish Oceans Institute (SOI) at St Andrews worked together on the project. They taught the seals to sing by playing sounds close to their natural vocal range, before rewarding them with treats when they successfully copied them. Dr Stansbury, who now works at El Paso Zoo in Texas, said: 'I was amazed how well the seals copied the model sounds we played to them. 'Copies were not perfect but given that these are not typical seal sounds it is pretty impressive. 'Our study really demonstrates how flexible seal vocalisations are. Previous studies just provided anecdotal evidence for this.' Professor Janik, Director of the SOI, said: 'This study gives us a better understanding of the evolution of vocal learning, a skill that is crucial for human language development.' He added: 'Surprisingly, non-human primates have very limited abilities in this domain. 'Finding other mammals that use their vocal tract in the same way as us to modify sounds informs us on how vocal skills are influenced by genetics and learning and can ultimately help to develop new methods to study speech disorders.' As seals separate from their mothers when they are just two or three weeks old, the findings suggest they could be used to study speech disorders and test different methods for slower learners, the researchers said. 'Since seals use the same neural and anatomical structures as humans to produce these sounds, they provide a good model system in which to study how speech sounds are learned,' Professor Janik said. 'As they separate so early from their mothers, we can control what exactly they hear when, which makes such studies much easier than with humans who are exchanging sounds with parents for all of their development all the time.' A seal called Hoover was documented copying human speech - including phrases like 'how are you?' - at the New England aquarium in Boston, Massachusetts in the 1980s. However, this does not necessarily mean that the mammals could learn to talk like humans. Professor Janik added: 'While seals can copy such sentences, they would not know what they mean. 'We would have to investigate whether they are able to label objects vocally, which is a key requirement for actually talking about things. 'Our study suggests that they have the production skills to produce human language. Whether they can make sense of it would be the next question.'


Times
4 hours ago
- Times
Twinkle-toed cockatoos have 30 different dance moves, researchers find
To most Australians they are mischievous, loud hooligans but researchers have found cockatoos also have an unexpected skill — they are also great dancers. The large, raucous parrots have an impressive 30 different dance moves — including headbanging, sidestepping and body rolls — according to researchers at Australia's Charles Sturt University. They found some individual birds had unique dance moves when hearing music, which often involved combining several movements in new ways. Known for stripping timber, raiding rubbish bins and destroying lawns, cockatoos are among the louts of the bird world but they are also highly intelligent. Not only did the researchers review hours of footage of the birds in the wild and in captivity, they also played the birds music, a podcast or simply left them in silence to see their reactions. Among the songs the cockatoos danced to was The Nights by the Swedish DJ Avicii. The researchers said that they chose 'progressive house' music for the birds to dance too, believing it unlikely the cockatoos had previously heard the songs. All the birds danced, even when there was no music to dance to. The scientists said that the findings showed that at least ten of the 21 known cockatoo species enjoy a boogie, and suggested that playing music to captive birds could help keep them healthy and happy. The study, published in the journal Plos One, said that cockatoo dancing results from complex brain processes including imitation, learning and synchronised, rhythmic movement. Spontaneous dancing in time to music has only been reported in humans and parrots, although some other wild birds also display rhythmic movements as part of their courtship displays, it said. However, what motivates captive birds to dance remains unclear. The researchers analysed 45 videos posted on social media that showed cockatoos dancing. They identified a total of 30 distinct dance movements — 17 of which had not previously been described scientifically. When the researchers then investigated dancing behaviour in cockatoos from three species housed at a New South Wales zoo by playing them music, a podcast or no soundtrack at all, they found that all the birds performed dance moves. Cockatoos, the study said, appeared to display a wide repertoire of dance moves, many of which were similar to the courtship displays of wild parrots. This suggested that their dancing abilities may have originated as courtship behaviour that has been redirected towards their owners when the birds are in captivity. 'The analysis also indicated that dancing is far more complex and varied than previously thought, recording 30 different movements seen in multiple birds and a further 17 movements that were seen in only one bird,' said the lead researcher, Natasha Lubke. 'As well as supporting the presence of positive emotions in birds and advancing dance behaviour as an excellent model to study parrot emotions, the work suggests that playing music to parrots may provide a useful approach to enrich their lives in captivity, with positive effects on their welfare.'


The Guardian
5 hours ago
- The Guardian
The Origin of Language by Madeleine Beekman review – the surprising history of speech
The story of human evolution has undergone a distinct feminisation in recent decades. Or, rather, an equalisation: a much-needed rebalancing after 150 years during which, we were told, everything was driven by males strutting, brawling and shagging, with females just along for the ride. This reckoning has finally arrived at language. The origins of our species' exceptional communication skills constitutes one of the more nebulous zones of the larger evolutionary narrative, because many of the bits of the human anatomy that allow us to communicate – notably the brain and the vocal tract – are soft and don't fossilise. The linguistic societies of Paris and London even banned talk of evolution around 1870, and the subject only made a timid comeback about a century later. Plenty of theories have been tossed into the evidentiary void since then, mainly by men, but now evolutionary biologist Madeleine Beekman, of the University of Sydney, has turned her female gaze on the problem. Her theory, which she describes as having been hiding in plain sight, is compelling: language evolved in parallel with caring for our 'underbaked' newborns, because looking after a creature as helpless as a human baby on the danger-filled plains of Africa required more than one pair of hands (and feet). It needed a group among whom the tasks of food-gathering, childcare and defence could be divided. A group means social life, which means communication. The evidence to support Beekman's theory isn't entirely lacking, though a lot of it is, necessarily, circumstantial. We know that the compromise that natural selection hit upon to balance the competing anatomical demands of bipedalism and an ever-expanding brain was to have babies come out early – before that brain and its bony casing were fully formed. One of the discoveries of the newly feminised wave of evolutionary science has been that alloparents – individuals other than the biological parents who contribute parenting services – played a critical role in ensuring the survival of those half-cooked human children. Another is that stone age women hunted alongside men. In the past it was assumed that hunting bands were exclusively male, and one theory held that language arose to allow them to cooperate. But childcare was another chore that called for cooperation, probably also between genders, and over years, not just hours or days. Luckily, the reconfiguration of the head and neck required to accommodate the ballooning brain had a side-effect of remoulding the throat, giving our ancestors more precise control over their utterances. With the capacity to generate a large range of sounds came the ability to convey a large range of meanings. To begin with, this was useful for coordinating childcare, but as speech became more sophisticated, alloparents – particularly grandmothers – used it to transmit their accumulated knowledge, thereby nurturing infants who were even better equipped to survive. The result of this positive feedback loop was Homo sapiens, the sole survivor of a once diverse lineage. Alas, Beekman takes a very long time to get to this exciting idea. She spends about half the book laying the groundwork, padding it out with superfluous vignettes as if she is worried the centre won't hold. Once she gets there, she makes some thought-provoking observations. Full-blown language probably emerged about 100,000 years ago, she thinks, but only in our line – not in those of our closest relatives. 'We may have made babies with Neanderthals and Denisovans,' she writes, 'but I don't think we had much to talk about.' And whereas others have argued that language must have predated Homo sapiens, because without it the older species Homo erectus couldn't have crossed the forbidding Wallace Line – the deep-water channel that separates Asia and Australasia – she draws on her deep knowledge of social insects to show that communication as relatively unsophisticated as that of bees or ants could have done the made a persuasive case for the role of alloparents in the evolution of language, Beekman concludes that we did ourselves a disservice when we shrank our basic unit of organisation down from the extended to the nuclear family. Maybe, but historians including Peter Laslett have dated this important shift to the middle ages, long before the Industrial Revolution where she places it, and the damage isn't obvious yet. Language is still being soaked up by young children; it's still a vehicle for intergenerational learning. It may take a village to raise a child, but as Beekman herself hints, a village can be constituted in different ways. Sign up to Inside Saturday The only way to get a look behind the scenes of the Saturday magazine. Sign up to get the inside story from our top writers as well as all the must-read articles and columns, delivered to your inbox every weekend. after newsletter promotion The Origin of Language: How We Learned to Speak and Why by Madeleine Beekman is published by Simon & Schuster (£25). To support the Guardian buy a copy at Delivery charges may apply.