Partial Solar Eclipse Ahead: Catch The Sun Turning Into 'Devil's Horns' On March 29
Here's a reason to wake up early on a Saturday: On March 29, a solar eclipse will grace the skies at sunrise over eastern Canada and the northeastern United States, as well as other northern locations.
It will be the first solar eclipse since last spring's 'Great American Eclipse' on April 8, 2024.
It won't be a total solar eclipse, but it'll be close. It's considered a 'deep partial solar eclipse,' and about 93% of the sun's disk will be obscured by the new moon.
(MORE: Colorado Skies Glow, But Not From Aurora)
Parts of North America will be able to see the eclipse at sunrise or shortly after, then Greenland, Iceland, Europe and northwest Africa will be able to view the eclipse as the sun climbs throughout the morning.
Russia will be last to view the eclipse, with Siberia getting the chance to see it at sunset.
Quebec will have the best view, with a maximum of 93.1% of the sun shadowed by the moon. The border between Maine and New Brunswick will have spectacular views as well, Live Science reported.
(MORE: 50 Baby Stars Celebrate Webb Telescope)
The farther south in the U.S. you are, the less coverage you'll see. Northern Maine could see up to 85% coverage, but in Philadelphia, only about 11% of the sun will appear covered. And in Washington, D.C., that number drops to 1%, according to Live Science. You can check the percentage visible from your location at in-the-sky.org.
In some locations, the sun will take on the appearance of devil's horns or a Cheshire Cat grin, Forbes reported. As the sun won't be completely eclipsed, it will look similar to a very bright, horizontal crescent moon.
However, according to weather.com senior digital meteorologist Chris Dolce, conditions in the Northeast are not looking optimal for viewing the eclipse. A weather disturbance may bring a decent amount of cloud cover that morning, and rain or snow are not out of the question.
If you plan to try and take a glance, don't forget to wear certified solar glasses to protect your eyes. Viewers can also use a telescope or a pair of binoculars with a solar filter. Failure to use the proper eye protection can cause serious eye damage.
Hashtags

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles
Yahoo
17 hours ago
- Yahoo
We're halfway between the April 2024 and August 2026 total solar eclipses: Here's why we're excited
When you buy through links on our articles, Future and its syndication partners may earn a commission. There may be no total solar eclipse this year, but June 10, 2025, is a landmark event of sorts for eclipse chasers worldwide — the halfway point between the last and the next totalities. It's 429 days — just over 14 months — since April 8, 2024's "Great American Eclipse" across North America and 429 days until the next total solar eclipse on August 12, 2026, over Greenland, Iceland and Spain. Related: Eclipse at sea: Best cruises for the total solar eclipse 2026 For some devotees of what's often called "nature's greatest event," it's a moment to reflect on and relive a landmark eclipse now in the rearview mirror. About 50 million people experienced the last total solar eclipse in Mexico, 15 U.S. states and southeastern Canada. It was, as expected by many, an event with mixed weather. Texas, which had the best odds of a clear sky, had clouds to varying degrees, with the clearest weather in the supposedly cloudiest regions of Indiana, Ohio, upstate New York, Vermont, New Hampshire and Maine in the U.S., and Quebec and New Brunswick in Canada. It was even cloudy over northern Mexico, but some had the right kind of sea legs. "We had to sail a considerable distance away from the coast to find a spot where it would be clear at eclipse time, and when the eclipse began in a clear blue sky, I got a rush of excitement as I realized we were going to see the entire spectacle," Rick Fienberg at the American Astronomical Society's Solar Eclipse Task Force, told During totality, he noticed two parallel contrails in the sky close to the eclipse. "I realized they were from the two NASA research planes that were chasing totality about 40,000 feet above us [and] when I swung my binoculars to them, I could see the jets themselves," Fienberg continued. Thoughts are now firmly fixed on the next total solar eclipse on August 12, 2026, which will see eclipse chasers head to the fjords of eastern Greenland in cruise ships, to remote parts of western Iceland and to northern Spain. The total solar eclipse on August 12, 2026, will have a maximum totality of 2 minutes and 18 seconds and be mainland Europe's first since 1999. Eclipse cartographer Michael Zeiler at and who was clouded out in Texas, has plans in place. "I am in great anticipation of the return of nature's greatest sight," Zeiler told "Total solar eclipses are guaranteed to take you to beautiful remote locations on Earth, and in 2026, Greenland is high on my bucket list." Bucket list locations abound across the path. "I don't think the tourism industry realizes what they are about to experience because we haven't had a total solar eclipse visible from Iceland since the summer of 1954," Sævar Helgi Bragason, an astronomy and science communicator at and told who's been preparing since 2015. "I've spent the better part of last year and this year increasing awareness with the general public, giving talks and having countless meetings with municipalities, tour companies, even the minister of justice and members of the parliament," he says. "It's finally paying off [but] we still have a long way to go." He's hoping Iceland's unique charms appeal to eclipse chasers, adding: "You could witness the eclipse from a geothermal pool or surrounded by incredible volcanic landscapes — and Icelanders know how to throw a good party!" For Spain, there's been a much longer wait for what will be a very special totality. "It's the first total solar eclipse visible from mainland Spain in over 120 years, and it's taking place in my home country," Dan Zafra, an astrophotographer at told "It's a rare opportunity not just for Spaniards but for eclipse chasers and photographers worldwide to witness totality in a stunning and accessible European setting." As eclipse chasers on cruise ships in Greenland experience totality, the sun and moon will be about 25 degrees above the northwestern horizon. However, once the path has crossed northern Spain, it will be close to sunset. Fienberg will be on a cruise ship in the western Mediterranean Sea between the coast of mainland Spain and Mallorca, one of the Balearic Islands. "We'll have a little more than 1½ minutes of totality with the sun about 3½ degrees above the horizon just before sundown," said Fienberg. "Then we'll see the thin crescent sun appear to sink into the sea. The entire spectacle should be quite dramatic." Since any one place on Earth experiences totality on average once every 36 years, according to NASA, eclipse chasing typically demands travel. "Once you've seen the dramatic beauty of a total solar eclipse, you might well get the bug to travel the world to see more," Zeiler said. "Life is short, and travel to far-away destinations will be so rewarding in so many ways." The 2026 total solar eclipse will be followed one lunar year later on August 2, 2027, when a maximum totality of 6 minutes and 23 seconds will be seen from Egypt, with Spain, Morocco, Algeria, Tunisia, Libya, Egypt, Saudi Arabia, Yemen and Somalia also in the path. Another lunar year will see a 5-minute and 10-second totality on July 22, 2028, in Australia, with New Zealand getting totality close to sunset. To the uninitiated, the passion of eclipse chasers leaves many confused. Perhaps that's because photos of totality typically fail to do the majesty of the event justice. Seeing the solar corona with the naked eye is a moment to savor, but there's much more to it than visuals." You don't just see a total solar eclipse. You feel it as the air cools and the wind changes speed and direction. You hear it in the cheers and cries of those around you and the sounds of insects and birds behaving as they do at sunset," said Fienberg. "It's among the most awesome experiences you can have on this planet — right up there with falling in love."
Yahoo
2 days ago
- Yahoo
How do migrating birds know where they're going?
When you buy through links on our articles, Future and its syndication partners may earn a commission. Every year, billions of birds migrate in and out of the United States. And across the world, birds fly thousands of miles to reach their seasonal destinations. Some birds, like the Arctic tern (Sterna paradisaea), even rack up enough miles over their lifetime to fly to the moon and back. But when birds embark on these epic journeys, how do they know where they're going? Birds have an arsenal of senses they use to orient themselves — some we're familiar with, and some are still beyond human comprehension. "We know that birds use a variety of cues to keep their migratory direction," Miriam Liedvogel, director of the Institute of Avian Research in Germany, told Live Science in an email. Sign up for our newsletter Sign up for our weekly Life's Little Mysteries newsletter to get the latest mysteries before they appear online. Sight and smell are two basic cues that birds use to find their way. If birds have already migrated once, they'll likely remember familiar landmarks, such as rivers and mountain ranges. Birds that migrate over water, on the other hand, have fewer landmarks to guide them. In these circumstances, they might rely more on their sense of smell; one study found that when researchers blocked the nasal passageways of seabirds called Scopoli's shearwaters (Calonectris diomedea), they could still fly over land but became disoriented when flying over water. Birds also can use the sun and stars as guides. To do this, birds that fly during the day use a "sun compass," which combines birds' view of where the sun is in the sky with their internal perception of what time of day it is based on their circadian rhythm. By integrating these two inputs, birds can determine the direction they're heading, like a living sundial. Research shows that disrupting a bird's circadian rhythm with artificial light prevents them from navigating accurately, showing the importance of the sun compass. However, most birds actually migrate at night, meaning the position of the sun is of little use to them. In this case, birds rely on the position and rotation of the stars to find their way. They use this star compass by learning the position of the stars around the celestial pole, which is roughly marked by Polaris (the North Star) — the same star humans have used to navigate for millennia. Related: Why don't all birds fly? But what if the sky is cloudy, and birds can't see the sun, stars or any landmarks? That's when birds' more fantastic senses come into play. Birds can find their way even with no sun or stars, partly thanks to a sense called magnetoreception. This sense allows birds to perceive Earth's magnetic fields, which are generated by the rippling molten metals in our planet's core. This feat may sound like science fiction, but research shows that messing with magnetic fields has a big effect on birds; for example, one study found that altering the magnetic fields around pigeons disrupted their homing abilities. While it's clear that birds are capable of magnetoreception, exactly how they do it is less certain. Peter Hore, a professor of chemistry at the University of Oxford, said birds must utilize some sort of chemical reaction whose outcome depends on the strength and direction of Earth's magnetic field. There are a couple of candidate theories for how this reaction happens, but Hore's bet is on a molecule called cryptochrome, which is present in birds' retinas. Researchers have confirmed in the lab that isolated cryptochrome responds to magnetic fields and that this reaction requires blue light, which has also been shown to be necessary for bird magnetoreception. Still, researchers aren't exactly sure how cryptochrome is sensitive enough to pick up on tiny variations in Earth's magnetic field. "We know so little about the details of how this compass might operate," Hore said. "I mean, we don't even know how many cryptochrome molecules there are in the birds' retinas." Some research also points to a magnetoreception mechanism inside birds' beaks. Studies have found receptors that interact with magnetite, an iron-based mineral, in the upper portion of birds' beaks. These receptors connect to the brain via important nerve pathways, suggesting they could be another technique birds use to gauge the intensity of the magnetic field. On top of magnetoreception, birds can gain information about their direction by detecting polarized light — a type of light in which the waves oscillate in a specific, aligned plane. Sunlight becomes polarized in predictable ways when light scatters through Earth's atmosphere. Using special cells in their retinas, birds can sense these patterns, which give them information about where the sun is in the sky, even when it's overcast. Just as we rely on our vision during the day but may use our hands to guide ourselves around a dimly lit room at night, birds employ different senses at different times. "Birds likely integrate their compass cues to navigate — and we are pretty sure that different cues are of varying importance during their journey," Liedvogel said. Hore pointed this out as well; magnetoreception, for example, is less useful during thunderstorms or periods of high solar activity, both of which can disrupt Earth's magnetic fields, he said. RELATED MYSTERIES —Are birds reptiles? —Why do hummingbirds 'hum'? —Why are there so many pigeons? Ultimately, all of these strategies are underpinned by birds' genetic drive to migrate. Birds inherit the propensity to migrate from their parents, Liedvogel explained, and the distance and direction in which they fly are primarily based on genetics. Researchers like Liedvogel are still investigating exactly which genes are responsible and how they work. Both scientists said understanding these systems will be essential to the future of bird conservation. Relocating or rewilding bird species has become a major focus of wildlife conservation efforts, but so far, outcomes have been mixed; one analysis found that in 45% of studies, the birds left their new location. "Human efforts to relocate those birds have not been very successful," Hore said. "That's partly because they are such good navigators that if you displace them, they simply fly back."
Yahoo
3 days ago
- Yahoo
Scientists Intrigued by Conical Skull Found in Ancient Burial Ground
Archaeologists in Iran have discovered an ancient cone-shaped skull that is believed to have belonged to a teen girl — and there are signs of tragedy in her bones. As Live Science reports, the skull, which was found in a prehistoric burial ground known as Chega Sofla without its corresponding skeleton, shows signs not only of intentional modification, but also possibly fatal blunt force trauma. Dated to roughly 6,200 years old, the strange cone shape of the skull appears to be the product of a practice archaeologists today call artificial cranial modification, a process similar to foot-binding in which the soft skulls of children are bandaged to deliberately deform them. Found across cultures and millennia, this type of body modification has been undertaken for various reasons, including to denote social status or adhere to beauty standards, as evidenced by it more often being seen in girls than boys. Though it's still occasionally practiced today, the practice sometimes referred to as "skull elongation" was far more common in prehistoric times. The girl with the conical skull in this study, for instance, was believed to have lived in the fifth millennium BCE. Aside from the cone-shaped cranium of the young woman, who was believed to be younger than the age of 20, archaeologists Mahdi Alirezazadeh and Hamed Vahdati Nasab of the Tarbiat Modares University in Tehran — who also authored a study about their discovery that was recently published in the International Journal of Osteoarcheology — also found a long, unhealed fracture on the back of the skull that likely killed her. "We know this woman experienced the fracture in the final moments of her life," Alirezazadeh told Live Science, "but we don't have any direct evidence to say that someone intentionally struck her." Though it's unclear whether the ancient teen in question was intentionally killed or died by accident, the researchers believe that the modified shape of her skull likely made it weaker and more susceptible to trauma than a conventional cranium. Along with pointing out that an unmodified fractured skull was found alongside the conical skull in the portion of Chega Sofia where they were working, Alirezazadeh also noted that whatever killed the latter "was so severe that it would have fractured a normal, unmodified skull as well." More on head science: Scientists Want You to Ink an Electronic Tattoo On Your Forehead So Your Boss Can Detect Your Mental State