logo
Chimps sometimes care for others' wounds, and scientists want to know why

Chimps sometimes care for others' wounds, and scientists want to know why

Washington Post14-05-2025

Primatologist Elodie Freymann arrived in Uganda's Budongo Forest in 2021 to observe the chimpanzees there and learn more about their ability to self-medicate with healing plants.
But as she flipped through a field book at the site containing the observations of researchers dating back to 1993, she began to notice accounts of the chimps not just ingesting plants to self-medicate, but using them for wound care — and sometimes not always on themselves.
In looking through 30 years of observations — as well as eight months of their own — Freymann and her colleagues found that the chimpanzees of Budongo have, in dozens of instances, administered first aid on themselves and others. According to Freymann, these observations, published Wednesday in Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, raise questions over chimpanzees' capacity for empathy and altruism.
'It's hard to prove that a nonhuman animal has empathy because you can't sit down and have a conversation with them,' Freymann said. 'Most studies have been done in captivity. But this provides a case study, or several case studies, of chimpanzees in the wild possessing the ability to not just take care of themselves, but to transfer those skills to others.'
Chimpanzees and their behaviors have long dominated the field of zoopharmacognosy, the study of nonhuman self-medication. In addition to Freymann's previous research looking into chimpanzees ingesting healing plants to self-medicate, researchers who documented chimpanzees using insects to treat themselves and others in Loango National Park in Gabon argued that their behavior was evidence of their capacity for 'prosocial behaviors,' or voluntary actions that serve the best interest of another.
The research into the chimpanzees of Budongo found 34 instances of the chimpanzees practicing self-care, whether it be something as simple as licking their wounds or using leaves to wipe after a bowel movement or mating, to something more complex, such as chewing up plants and putting the material on a wound. There have been seven more instances of chimpanzees providing this sort of care on other chimpanzees and not just related kin.
In 2012, a subadult male — a chimpanzee between the age of 10 and 14 — identified as PS sucked the wound on the leg of another subadult male identified as ZG. In 2008, researchers documented an adult male identified as NK removing the nylon snare off an unrelated adult female.
The behavior also raises questions over whether the animals' caring skills are instinctual or acquired and then passed on through social learning, Freymann said. In 2008, researchers observed an adult female identified as NB, injured in a bout of intragroup aggression, applying a folded and chewed leaf to her wound. Her daughter, a juvenile female identified as NT, observed her mother doing this and then mimicked the behavior, chewing a leaf and then applying it to her mother's wound.
'I'm not making a case that every certain medicinal behavior is learned, but I think it's not out of the question that chimpanzees are capable of possessing medicinal culture,' she said.
Given that apes are considered the closest evolutionary cousin to human beings, understanding 'cognitive and social foundations of health-care behaviors in humans requires examining their evolutionary precursors in our closest living relatives,' Freymann argued in her research article. The chimpanzee behavior observed in Budongo suggests that 'the shared ancestors that we have with chimps and apes would have likely been capable of this kind of caregiving and have the capacity to identify those in need of care and to provide that to others,' Freymann said.
But Alexander Piel, a University College London associate professor of evolutionary anthropology who was not involved in Freymann's research, noted that the prosocial behavior demonstrated by chimpanzees in Budongo was very rare, which makes him reluctant to tie the findings to human evolution. There's always an inclination, he said, to connect similarities between humans and apes to evolution, and while it's always a possibility, 'we share other features with them, like our intense sociality, and we share them with non-apes.'
Researchers have observed prosocial health-care behaviors among non-ape species such as elephants and dwarf mongooses, Freymann wrote in her study. 'The fact that we see them in non-closely related species suggests that there are some other drivers to this beyond humanness or human-relatedness. … Empathy is a part of the equation, but the data doesn't support that it's the ultimate driver of this behavior,' Piel said.
The findings provide a good jumping-off point, Piel said, for exploration into the drivers behind this rare type of prosocial behavior — for example, why chimps are so selective in providing health care to other chimps. Freymann said there will be more studies to come, as well as more long-term monitoring.
'I think we're going to find medicinal cultures not just in chimps but in other animals as well,' Freymann said. 'There's debate always, but there are some medicinal behaviors that appear to be instinctual, the more basic ones. There are some behaviors that we've observed that I think are too complex to be instinctual.'

Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

First evidence of ‘living towers' made of worms discovered in nature
First evidence of ‘living towers' made of worms discovered in nature

CNN

time2 hours ago

  • CNN

First evidence of ‘living towers' made of worms discovered in nature

Nature seems to offer an escape from the hustle and bustle of city life, but the world at your feet may tell another story. Even in the shade of a fruit tree, you could be surrounded by tiny skyscrapers — not made of steel or concrete, but of microscopic worms wriggling and writhing into the shape of long, vertical towers. Even though these miniature architects, called nematodes, are found all over Earth's surface, scientists in Germany recently witnessed their impressive building techniques in nature for the first time. After months of closely inspecting rotten pears and apples in local orchards, researchers from the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior and the University of Konstanz were able to spot hundreds of the 1-millimeter-long (0.04-inch) worms climbing onto one another, amassing structures up to 10 times their individual size. Related video Rare video shows 12 sharks co-feed socially To learn more about the mysterious physics of the soft, slimy towers, the study team brought samples of nematodes called Caenorhabditis elegans into a lab and analyzed them. There, the scientists noticed the worms could assemble in a matter of hours, with some reaching out from the twisting mass as exploratory 'arms' sensing the environment and building accordingly. But why the worms formed the structures wasn't immediately clear. The team's findings, published Thursday in the journal Current Biology, show that even the smallest animals can prompt big questions about the evolutionary purpose of social behaviors. 'What we got was more than just some worms standing on top of each other,' said senior study author Serena Ding, a Max Planck research group leader of genes and behavior. 'It's a coordinated superorganism, acting and moving as a whole.' To find out what was motivating the nematodes' building behavior, the study team tested the worms' reactions to being poked, prodded and even visited by a fly — all while stacked in a tower formation. 'We saw that they are very reactive to the presence of a stimulus,' said the study's first author, Daniela Perez, who is a postdoctoral researcher at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior. 'They sense it, and then the tower goes towards this stimulus, attaching itself to our metal pick or a fly buzzing around.' This coordinated reaction suggests the hungry nematodes may be joining together to easily hitch a ride on larger animals such as insects that transport them to (not so) greener pastures with more rotten fruit to feast on, Perez said. 'If you think about it, an animal that is 1 millimeter long cannot just crawl all the way to the next fruit 2 meters (6.6 feet) away. It could easily die on the way there, or be eaten by a predator,' Perez explained. Nematodes are capable of hitchhiking solo too, she added, but arriving to a new area in a group may allow them to continue reproducing. The structures themselves may also serve as a mode of transport, as evidenced by how some worms formed bridges across gaps within the petri dishes to get from one surface to another, Perez noted. 'This discovery is really exciting,' said Orit Peleg, an associate professor of computer science who studies living systems at the University of Colorado Boulder's BioFrontiers Institute. 'It's both establishing the ecological function of creating a tower, and it really opens up the door to do more controlled experimentation to try to understand the perceptual world of these organisms, and how they communicate within a large group.' Peleg was not involved in the study. As the next step, Perez said her team would like to learn whether the formation of these structures is a cooperative or competitive behavior. In other words, are the towering nematodes behaving socially to help each other out, or are their towers more akin to a Black Friday sale stampede? Studying the behaviors of other self-assembling creatures could offer clues to the social norms of nematodes and help answer this question, Ding said. Ants, which assemble to form buoyant rafts to survive floodwaters, are among the few creatures known to team up like nematodes, said David Hu, a professor of mechanical engineering and biology at Georgia Tech. Hu was not involved in the study. 'Ants are incredibly sacrificial for one another, and they do not generally fight within the colony,' Hu said. 'That's because of their genetics. They all come from the same queen, so they are like siblings.' Like ants, nematodes didn't appear to display any obvious role differentiation or hierarchy within the tower structures, Perez said. Each worm from the base to the top of the structure was equally mobile and strong, indicating no competition was at play. However, the lab-cultivated worms were basically clones of one another, so it's not clear whether role differentiation occurs more often in nature, where nematode populations could have more genetic differences, she noted. Additionally, socially cooperative creatures tend to use some form of communication, Peleg said. In the case of ants, it may be their pheromone trails, while honeybees rely on their ritual dance routines and slime molds use their pulsing chemical signals. With nematodes, however, it's still not clear how they might communicate — or if they are communicating at all, Ding said. 'The next steps for (the team) are really just choosing the next questions to ask.' Notably, there has been a lot of interest in studying cooperative animal behaviors among the robotics community, Hu said. It's possible that one day, he added, information about the complex sociality of creatures like nematodes could be used to inform how technology, such as computer servers or drone systems, communicates.

GLP-1 RAs Linked to Doubled Risk for Macular Degeneration
GLP-1 RAs Linked to Doubled Risk for Macular Degeneration

Medscape

time3 hours ago

  • Medscape

GLP-1 RAs Linked to Doubled Risk for Macular Degeneration

Patients with diabetes using glucagon-like peptide 1 receptor agonists (GLP-1 RAs) may face more than twice the risk for developing neovascular age-related macular degeneration compared with those not using the medications, data from Canada showed. The risk appears to increase with longer exposure to the drugs. METHODOLOGY: Researchers conducted a population-based, retrospective cohort study using administrative data for 139,002 patients aged 66 years or older in Ontario, Canada, from January 2020 to November 2023. The researchers matched 46,334 patients who had received a GLP-1 agent (predominantly semaglutide) for at least 6 months 1:2 with patients who had not received this type of medication, based on factors such as age, gender, duration of diabetes, and comorbidities. They examined the incidence of new diagnoses of neovascular age-related macular degeneration during 3 years of follow-up. TAKEAWAY: Patients exposed to GLP-1 RAs showed a higher incidence of neovascular age-related macular degeneration than unexposed patients (0.2% vs 0.1%). Both unadjusted and adjusted Cox proportional hazard models estimated higher risk for neovascular age-related macular degeneration among patients exposed to GLP-1 medication (hazard ratio [HR], 2.11; 95% CI, 1.58-2.82; adjusted HR, 2.21; 95% CI, 1.65-2.96). In a sensitivity analysis, longer duration of exposure to a GLP-1 RA was associated with increased risk for neovascular age-related macular degeneration. IN PRACTICE: 'GLP-1 RAs have had a tremendous role in the care of patients with diabetes and now those needing additional help with weight management. However, the adage that 'there is no such thing as a free lunch' remains true,' wrote Brian L. VanderBeek, MD, MPH, with the Scheie Eye Institute at the University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, in an editorial accompanying the study. 'While certainly not outweighing the good these medications offer, prescribing physicians need to keep in mind the real and serious ocular adverse events that may occur.' SOURCE: Rajeev H. Muni, MD, with St. Michael's Hospital in Toronto, Ontario, was the corresponding author of the study, which was published online on June 5 in JAMA Ophthalmology . LIMITATIONS: The analysis did not stratify results by the type of GLP-1 RA prescribed or consider dose, route of administration, or frequency of administration. The researchers had limited information about variables like smoking and sun exposure. 'Further research is needed to determine if there is a true cause and effect relationship,' the study authors wrote. DISCLOSURES: Some of the researchers have received support from the Silver Target Foundation, the PSI Foundation, and Fighting Blindness Canada. Muni declared consulting for Alcon, Apellis, AbbVie, Bayer, Bausch Health, and Roche. His institution has received financial support from these companies.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into the world of global news and events? Download our app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store