Subtle differences identified in brains of people with schizophrenia
Schizophrenia might be among the most intensely studied mental illnesses. However, its causes—and the way those causes manifest in the brain—remain frustratingly elusive. A new study published February 26 in the American Journal of Psychiatry examines physical differences between the brains of people who experience schizophrenia and those who do not.
Wolfgang Omlor, a study co-author and psychiatrist from the University of Zurich in Switzerland, tells Popular Science that 'while schizophrenia may have its own mechanisms shaping brain structure and function, these processes remain far from fully understood.'
To identify trends in physical features common to the brains of people with schizophrenia, Omlor and his team combed through data from the global Enhancing Neuroimaging Genetics Through Meta-Analysis (ENIGMA) database covering 6,037 people. They saw two potential features: an increased uniformity of folding patterns in a part of the brain called the right caudal anterior cingulate region, and an increased variability in the thickness of the cerebral cortex.
An immediate question is whether these features could contribute to causing schizophrenia, or whether they're caused by the condition. Similarly, it's not clear whether these features are innate, or develop over the course of a person's life. Omlor explains that because this study is 'comparing individuals already diagnosed with schizophrenia to controls,' it can't address the question of causation.
As far as whether these features are something with which a person is born, or whether they develop over the course of life, he says, 'It's likely a combination of both. Some features may be present from early development, while others can emerge later, influenced by factors like medication or chronic stress that reshape the brain through neuroplasticity.'
According to Omlor, cortical folding appears to fall into the category of features present from early development and is mostly established before birth and into early childhood. However, schizophrenia itself tends to manifest in late adolescence/early adulthood. This raises the possibility that these folding patterns represent some sort of predisposition to schizophrenia; whether or not this results in the onset of the actual condition, however, will depend on any number of other factors.
The folding of cerebral tissue is one of the brain's most distinctive features. In general, 'folding increases the brain's surface area in limited space and is thought to influence how neurons connect,' Omlor says. Subtle differences in the nature of the way that brain tissue is folded are also a feature of other conditions, although it's difficult to identify what exact effect such differences have.
'Some neurodevelopmental conditions (e.g., autism) also show altered cortical folding,' he says. 'The implications of such differences aren't fully understood, but the more uniform folding in schizophrenia's anterior cingulate could point to a more constrained developmental process in that region.'
Exactly how these folding patterns might interact with the development and nature of the condition remains an open question, but Omlor theorizes that 'the uniform folding patterns may reflect a less flexible interplay between genes and environment at these earlier stages [of development].'
It certainly seems notable that the folds manifest in the right caudal anterior cingulate region, a part of the brain that integrates cognitive and emotional processes. However, a single role should not be given part of the brain.
'Brain regions generally work together in complex networks.So while we do see more uniform folding [in the right caudal anterior cingulate region] in schizophrenia, it's best not to think of [that region] as solely 'responsible' for any single task,' he says.'Rather, it's part of a broader circuit linking thought and emotion.'
The study's second finding is a 'greater variability in cortical thickness' in individuals with schizophrenia. As an example of how nothing related to the brain is ever simple, this isn't merely a question of people with schizophrenia having 'bigger' or 'smaller' cerebral cortices. Instead, one person's cortex might be thinner in some parts and thicker in others, while another might be just the opposite—so even though those two people's cortices might have the same overall volume, their internal make-up might be quite different. In people with schizophrenia, there is a broader spread of such variation in thickness.
The subtleties involved here reflect the fact that the brain's dizzying complexity means it's rarely possible to draw simple conclusions. Even in this case, where a basic interpretation might be that schizophrenia is linked to the volume of the cerebral cortex, the answer is not that straightforward.
'Greater variability of cortical thickness in schizophrenia reflects a broader range beyond a 'more' or 'less' pattern, underscoring the disorder's complexity,' says Omlor.
However, it does appear possible to correlate these differences with the myriad ways in which schizophrenia presents.
'We also found that variations in specific brain regions correlate with schizophrenia symptom domains, suggesting these structural differences mirror the diverse presentations of the condition,' Omlor says.
Ultimately, one key takeaway from this study is that the way in which schizophrenia presents in a person is as unique as that person themselves. Similarly, it appears that there's no single way in which schizophrenia can be reflected in a person's brain. If anything, there's perhaps a set of regions in which the condition manifests, but the ways in which it can do so appear to be many and varied.
Approaching schizophrenia in this individualistic manner isn't merely an abstract consideration. Instead, a better understanding of how and why the condition varies from person to person could 'guide more individualized precision-medicine efforts… by recognizing these differences early on, clinicians and researchers can work toward treatments better suited to each person's unique profile,' according to Omlor.
Hashtags

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles
Yahoo
2 hours ago
- Yahoo
Worm towers are all around us
Biologists estimate that four out of five animals on Earth are nematodes (AKA roundworms).The tiny, wriggling, transparent invertebrates are the most abundant creatures on the planet and are found nearly everywhere–from permafrost to the deep ocean. More than one million species make up this ubiquitous group, which includes parasites, decomposers, predators, and more. 'They're not about to take over the world, because they already did,' says Serena Ding, a biologist at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior in Konstanz, Germany tells Popular Science. 'Global worming has already happened.' Yet despite their ubiquity in the environment and in research labs (where the nematode C. elegans is a common model organism), a new discovery highlights that there's still a lot left to learn about these worms. Humble roundworms put cheerleaders' pyramid-building skills to shame. In order to disperse and explore their environment, wild nematodes self-assemble into tower-shaped superorganisms, according to a new study led by Ding and published June 5 in the journal Current Biology. Together, groups of the one-millimeter long worms can act as hyper-coordinated construction squads, with their living bodies providing the raw material for functional, temporary structures. It's a biological feat that even humans struggle to accomplish. This is also the first time that scientists have formally documented the nematode phenomenon occurring in nature. Worm towers– sometimes called swarms– 'were kind of just whispered about in the worm community,' says Ding. Previously, there were anecdotal reports and documented observations of these multi-nematode assemblages in labs and other artificial settings, but it wasn't certain if the formations happened naturally. Now, it's clear that nematodes do, indeed, form towers without artificial interference.'They do exist at high densities, they're interacting, and they're doing something together.' says Ding, who studies collective behavior. 'This was the most exciting thing for me.' One tower can contain thousands of worms in a single aggregation, which looks like a cohesive drop of wiggly gelatin. The impressive team effort enables nematodes to hitch rides on passing insects to more favorable habitats and bridge otherwise untraversable gaps, Ding and her co-authors suggest. Studying this behavior could offer insights into the evolution of social animals and how group decision making unfolds. Only a handful of organisms are known to form collective assemblages for the purpose of dispersing, similar to the nematode towers. 'It's actually super rare,' says Ding, noting that there are just three other, well-documented examples. Slime molds, which are technically single-celled amoebas, often take on multicellular forms, aggregating to make fruiting bodies that send out spores or moving from place to place in a group. Fire ants are known to form rafts with their bodies to get through flood waters, and arrange themselves into towers and bridges to navigate the landscape. Groups of spider mites weave themselves up with silk into a ball that can be carried to distant frontiers on the wind. With the new findings, nematodes gain membership to an exclusive group of evolutionary odd-balls. But though superorganism behavior is uncommon across the tree of life, worm towers themselves are surprisingly commonplace. To track them down in the wild, the researchers didn't have to travel far. They started by looking at fallen fruit beneath trees on their university campus with a digital microscope. On rotting apples and pears, they found dozens of nematode towers wiggling at the edges and points of the fruits' fleshy topography. The scientists also documented the behavior among nematodes found at a mushroom farm. Then, they devised a method of reliably recreating it in the lab. Here's their recipe for encouraging worm towers: place a few thousand nematodes on a food-free petri dish that's flat except for a single tooth brush bristle pointing upwards. Then, wait for a couple of hours. It's that simple. In hundreds of trials, the worms clustered into their writhing tower formation around the bristle more than 90 percent of the time. The longest towers in these experiments were well over a centimeter long (more than 10x a nematode's body length). Prior observations have noted towers about five centimeters–or almost two inches–high. In additional experiments with fruit flies and with a plastic probe, the researchers showed that worm towers strategically move towards any object that touches them or brushes by. The quick collective action allows the towers to shift fast enough to glom onto the leg of a passing insect. Previous research has documented individual nematodes hitchhiking on insects. However, through tower building, it seems that hundreds of worms can grab a ride at once, making the unwitting bug more akin to a subway train than a single passenger vehicle. The scientists also recorded two instances of the towers probing around and forming bridges to reach new locations, like the petri dish lid. Both observations support the leading hypothesis that nematodes build towers to access new, more suitable habitats. Using worms tagged with a fluorescent protein, Ding and her colleagues further found that nematodes building a tower all tend to orient themselves in the same direction. The worms point their heads upwards, and their bodies undulate in time with one another. Yet how they coordinate this intricate collaboration remains unclear. Many of the new observations prompt more confusion than clarity. For instance, in the wild groups, towers were exclusively made up of larvae. In the lab, nematodes of all ages collaborated to build. What accounts for the age difference is unknown. Ding and her colleagues didn't note any apparent competition for the top spots at the tip of the tower, where a worm is most likely to catch a ride. But it's unclear if a less genetically homogenous group of worms might be more competitive. The researchers also don't yet understand why the worms opt to disperse collectively instead of solo. Nor do they know how worms decide to begin forming a tower or the neural or sensory mechanisms that enable it. The basic physics of how thousands of tiny, slimy bodies manage to form something so coordinated and solid remains unresolved as well. 'There are the sorts of questions that we want to address,' says Ding. Ultimately, she hopes to use nematode towers to better understand animal cooperation across species. For so long in science, nematodes have been seen as little more than a microbiology model system, allowing researchers to test genetic modifications, understand cells, and map neurons. But, in looking so closely at the worms, generations of scientists may have missed the bigger picture. Nematodes have complex behaviors that are the product of millions of years of evolution. Learning more about what they do could shed light on how animals large and small work together, says Ding. 'It pays a lot to think about them as real animals,' she adds. 'They're everywhere, they're important, and they do things.'
Yahoo
3 hours ago
- Yahoo
Australia's giant extinct marsupials may be ID'd using tiny bone bits
While fictional depictions of fossil discoveries often feature someone finding an entire dinosaur bone in their backyard, the reality of paleontology usually involves puzzling over disparate fragments of a skeleton. Yet despite this often incomplete picture, even a tiny shard of bone can reveal new information—but only if scientists can be sure of exactly which species it belongs to. So how exactly do scientists determine where such fragments come from? One relatively new method for studying ancient bones is palaeoproteomics, the study of preserved proteins. A study published June 3 in Frontiers in Mammal Science describes the first successful use of this type of technique to characterize the bones of extinct species of Australian marsupial megafauna–the giant ancestors of today's Australian marsupials. The team took successful collagen samples from three such species: Protemnodon mamkurra, a giant kangaroo-like creature that is distantly related to today's kangaroos; Zygomaturus trilobus, a member of the extinct Diprotodontidae family that resembles a colossal wombat the size of a present day hippo; and Palorchestes azael, an unusual herbivore that bears a vague resemblance to a tapir. [ Related: Giant wombats the size of small cars once roamed Australia. ] Carli Peters of the Universidade do Algarve's MATRIX Project and one of the paper's co-authors, tells Popular Science that the research involved the use of zooarchaeology by mass spectrometry, or ZooMS. 'ZooMS is … based on small differences in the main bone protein, collagen type I, between species/genera/families,' she says. These differences allow for the identification of a collagen 'fingerprint,' which can then be compared to a sample from an as-yet-unidentified bone. If the fingerprints match, you can conclude that the bones are from the same species. Study co-author and archaeological scientist Katerina Douka explains that this technique can be used to sort through large numbers of fragments in a way that is impractical for other methods such as DNA sequencing. 'Such screening is not practical using DNA due to library preparation and sequencing costs and computational power needed,' she tells Popular Science But while collagen is hardy and generally less susceptible to degradation over long timescales than DNA, Australia's harsh environment has nevertheless made finding usable samples difficult. 'Fossils deposited in hot, dry and arid places, such as large parts of Australia, lose their collagen very early…. The major challenge [has been] discovering bones that contain [enough] collagen to allow us to apply such type analyses,' Douka says, The bones used for the study were found in the country's relatively temperate southeast and were initially used for radiocarbon dating and stable isotope analysis of other specimens. This research revealed the presence of preserved collagen, and the team jumped on the opportunity to study them. As Douka points out, 'This is the first time that such markers [have] become available for Australian megafauna.' This new data immediately opens doors for new research. 'We could identify these species at sites where they were previously not found, furthering our understanding of the past geographic range of these animals,' says Peters. [ Related: Super-muscular 374-pound kangaroos once thumped around Australia and New Guinea. ] However, there remain many, many more species for which no markers currently exist. These include some of the most fascinating and iconic of Australia's megafauna. These include various members of the genus Diprotodon, the largest marsupial genus to have ever existed, and Thylacoleo carnifex, the 'marsupial lion' that emerged roughly 2 million years ago during the late Pleistocene and was the continent's apex predator for millennia. Like the rest of Australia's megafauna, Thylacoleo carnifex disappeared around 50,000 years ago. Why? No one knows exactly why, and while the leading theory is that humans were to blame–the extinctions of the last megafauna coincide roughly with the arrival of humans in Australia–any new research carries the tantalizing possibility of shedding more light on how these species met their ultimate demise. Douka hopes that new tools like ZooMS might be able to 'help confirm or deny suggestions that early Aboriginals co-existed with megafauna in Australia.'


Boston Globe
20 hours ago
- Boston Globe
‘Opportunistic' cockatoos wait in line, drink from fountains, study finds
The 'drinking-fountain innovation' is the second time researchers have found the birds adapting their behavior to suit their environment, following a 'bin-opening innovation' recorded by the same team in the city's south, where birds were found to open the lids of household trash cans to access food waste. Lucy Aplin, an associate professor at the Australian National University and the University of Zurich, said the study was carried out as part of the Clever Cockie project, aimed at understanding how city living can drive behavioral change and social sharing. She said cockatoos are an excellent case study, as they are 'opportunistic and successful' in human environments, while Australia's relatively short history of urbanization means that evolutionary changes can be effectively ruled out. Advertisement 'Cockatoos are fantastic urban adapters,' Aplin said. 'They're also the sort of elephant of the bird world. They're very long-lived, famously. They're also very slow-breeding and very intelligent, and so they're not the sort of species you would expect to be an automatic urban adapter, but they are.' Advertisement Aplin said her colleague Barbara C. Klump, a behavioral ecologist at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior who led the research on the drinking-fountain study, 'literally stumbled' across cockatoos drinking from a fountain while out for a walk in western Sydney. Their team homed in on a 618-acre reserve in western Sydney made up of native vegetation, sports fields and, playgrounds that was home to a 'night roost' of about 100 to 150 birds. Researchers mapped 10 drinking fountains in the area and temporarily marked 24 cockatoos with identifying colors, which they estimate accounted for 16 to 24 percent of the local population. They also tagged two other individuals to gain information on long-term movements. From August to October 2019, they installed two cameras with a view of the fountain where cockatoos had been seen drinking. Known in Australia as a 'bubbler,' the fountain was made of a concrete stand about three feet high with a spring-loaded handle that needs to be twisted to operate. Footage over 44 days showed 525 attempts at drinking, with researchers estimating that 70 percent of the local population tried to drink from the fountain. Overall, birds were successful 41 percent of the time, with those who tried to access the water for longer, or with fewer other birds present, more likely to be successful. Footage from the cameras showed 'extensive queuing' before the birds gripped the handle of the fountain and twisted it with their feet, while also leaning their weight on the valve to ensure the water came out. Researchers found the number of attempts at drinking and success rate of the birds remained steady over the observation period, but a clear pattern emerged of birds coming to drink at 7:30 a.m. and 5:30 p.m. Weather also impacted the visitation rate, with fewer attempts when it was raining, the team found. Advertisement Aplin said it is a case study of an innovation that has 'arisen in response to an urban resource that's been provided by humans and then has spread through the local cockatoo population to establish as a tradition.' The data also suggests the fountain drinking is more widespread among the local population than the garbage-bin-opening behavior was at first, suggesting it had already undergone 'extensive social diffusion' before the study took place, researchers said. Unlike the trash-opening study, where the practice was more common in males, researchers said there was no sex bias in attempts to use or success at using the fountain, suggesting perhaps that a sense of innovation is not varied by sex but rather by the resources of the bird— with bin opening requiring more strength and therefore being suited to larger birds. The team also observed a range of techniques used to open the fountain, suggesting individual learning plays a role and possibly that those that have not yet mastered it are trying different methods. It's the latest of findings that show how animals are adapting to urban environments around the world. Already, scientists have documented great tits and blue tits in Britain piercing the tops of milk bottles to drink their contents, bullfinches opening sugar packets in Barbados, and long-tailed macaques bartering objects for food in Indonesia. Urban parrots are renowned for being particularly successful adapters despite being threatened by habitat loss and the wildlife trade, researchers said, with rosy-faced lovebirds in Phoenix found to use air-conditioning vents to cool off. Advertisement Aplin said her team is calling on citizen scientists to report instances of unusual cockatoo behavior for further investigation. She is already interested in following up reports of cockatoos knocking on windows for food and opening children's school bags to steal from their lunchboxes. 'In Australia, the kids often leave their bags outside the classroom, and then the cockatoos have ready access to work down the line of bags and pull all the lunchboxes out,' she said, adding that some schools have adapted their policies in response. 'I get a lot of these reports from the public now, and it's fascinating to see the variety of behavioral flexibility and innovations that these birds come up with.'