
Hold your horses! Now scientists say our equine friends CAN understand what we're talking about
Scientists have found that horses eavesdrop on human conversations, then store up the titbits they've gathered.
In a novel experiment, nosey nags tended to change their feeding habits after watching two people discussing eating a carrot.
The research by St Andrews University suggests that horses may understand far more than we give them credit for.
The study – published in the journal Animal Cognition – concludes: 'This indicates, for the first time, that some change their feeding strategies after eavesdropping on human-human demonstrations.
'The unintentional impact of humans on creating interspecies communication… needs to be reconsidered.'
The research is a collaboration between St Andrews equine psychology expert Kate Farmer – author of Thinking Horse and The Harmony Project – and Nürtingen-Geislingen University in Germany.
During the study, 17 horses were familiarised with a choice of two different buckets to feed from.
Researchers noted which tub – yellow or blue – the animals favoured.
Then, to see if horses would eavesdrop, each was allowed to witness from behind a fence an interaction between two humans in a riding arena.
They watched a woman help herself to a carrot from one bucket and begin eating it. She was then either praised or scolded by a second woman known to the horses.
The researchers made sure that the telling-off happened at each animal's favourite bucket.
After six repeats, the women left the arena and the horse was allowed in to choose which tub to feed from.
The process was repeated for eight days on the trot – after which 16 of the 17 horses were mostly feeding out of the 'good' bucket.
Of those, 12 had significantly changed their habits, including five who now entirely shunned the 'naughty' tub.
Animals who stabled together and lived more social lives were better able to pick up the signals.
It is already well known that horses can respond to human commands as well as following the direction of someone gazing or pointing.
And they have even been taught to pick out symbols to pass on the message that they want a rug on or off.
But this research suggests they also share the intelligence of other animals who have shown an ability to develop new habits after watching and listening to humans.
The study states: 'Such social learning has been discussed for Japanese macaques [monkeys], which demonstrate the washing of potatoes before eating them.'
Previous eavesdropping experiments involving dogs show that they warm towards people who hand out praise or have been rewarded for good behaviour, but they still relied on those people remaining present.
Jessica Pryor, who runs her own 'intelligent horsemanship' practice in Crosshill, Ayrshire, said: 'This study shows horses are capable of seeing and learning a lot more than they've been given credit for.
'It should engender respect for them, more for who they are than what they are.
'Not so long ago, horses were considered just things that did a job, then they were leisure animals that had to make their owners money.
'But they are actually smart, social and always switched on to what's going on round about them.
'Here, we can see them problem solving and asking themselves whether they have the right answer to the question.'
She added: 'We ask horses to do so many bizarre things and they trust us to be the leader.
'Thankfully, there's a large body of people now keeping horses more naturally than they have been – they're not supposed to be alone in a stable 23 hours a day.
'It was interesting in the study that the horses who lived together picked up the cues more easily.
'In a group, matriarchal mares are very good at teaching the youngsters good manners.'
The research will doubtless be of interest to Britain's horse lovers, which includes everyone from Sir Alex Ferguson to Queen Camilla.
The research also confirms the close bond between humans and horses that equestrians have long espoused. This relationship was famously explored in Hollywood movie The Horse Whisperer, starring Robert Redford.
In the 1998 film, Redford's cowboy character displays an almost magical ability to calm unruly horses.
It was said to be based on the real-life story of Dan 'Buck' Brannaman, whose training technique for difficult or troubled horses comes from the so-called 'natural horsemanship' movement.
Instead of 'breaking' horses, Brannaman communicates with them and tries to envisage the world from the horse's point of view.
Hashtags

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles


Scotsman
4 days ago
- Scotsman
Why Loch Ness Monster sceptics need to come up with another excuse for sightings
Sign up to our daily newsletter – Regular news stories and round-ups from around Scotland direct to your inbox Sign up Thank you for signing up! Did you know with a Digital Subscription to The Scotsman, you can get unlimited access to the website including our premium content, as well as benefiting from fewer ads, loyalty rewards and much more. Learn More Sorry, there seem to be some issues. Please try again later. Submitting... We all know what the Loch Ness Monster looks like, even if we've never actually seen it ourselves. Clearly, it is a serpentine creature that undulates across water, creating three or so loops above the surface. However, according to 'science', this would be a 'biological impossibility'. Quod erat demonstrandum, Nessie doesn't exist and it's all just made-up nonsense. Case closed. Advertisement Hide Ad Advertisement Hide Ad Or is it? According to new research by St Andrews University, only 1.5 per cent of possible Nessie sightings talk about seeing loops or hoops, suggesting the people making the reports have not been influenced by traditional imagery dating back to the 1500s. A famous photograph of the Loch Ness Monster from 1934, which later turned out to be a hoax (Picture: Keystone) | Getty Images 'This insight supports the contention that the majority of eyewitness reports are actually based on some underlying physical reality,' the researchers wrote before adding, rather disappointingly, 'even if not representing an actual encounter with an unknown species'.


Scotsman
4 days ago
- Scotsman
Why Loch Ness Monster sceptics need to come up with another excuse for sightings
Sign up to our daily newsletter – Regular news stories and round-ups from around Scotland direct to your inbox Sign up Thank you for signing up! Did you know with a Digital Subscription to The Scotsman, you can get unlimited access to the website including our premium content, as well as benefiting from fewer ads, loyalty rewards and much more. Learn More Sorry, there seem to be some issues. Please try again later. Submitting... We all know what the Loch Ness Monster looks like, even if we've never actually seen it ourselves. Clearly, it is a serpentine creature that undulates across water, creating three or so loops above the surface. However, according to 'science', this would be a 'biological impossibility'. Quod erat demonstrandum, Nessie doesn't exist and it's all just made-up nonsense. Case closed. Advertisement Hide Ad Advertisement Hide Ad Or is it? According to new research by St Andrews University, only 1.5 per cent of possible Nessie sightings talk about seeing loops or hoops, suggesting the people making the reports have not been influenced by traditional imagery dating back to the 1500s. A famous photograph of the Loch Ness Monster from 1934, which later turned out to be a hoax (Picture: Keystone) | Getty Images 'This insight supports the contention that the majority of eyewitness reports are actually based on some underlying physical reality,' the researchers wrote before adding, rather disappointingly, 'even if not representing an actual encounter with an unknown species'.

The National
25-06-2025
- The National
Capercaillie numbers double in parts of Scotland, study finds
With just over 500 of the ground-nesting birds left in the wild, the capercaillie, a large woodland grouse, is in danger of extinction in the UK. One contributor to the birds' decline is the eating of eggs and chicks by predators – including the pine marten, another protected species. READ MORE: Scottish Enterprise to keep funding Israel-linked arms firms, Kate Forbes says In an effort to protect capercaillie broods while not harming the predators themselves, researchers at the University of St Andrews used a 'diversionary feeding' scheme to give predators an easy alternative food to capercaillie nests. The study, which was carried out in and around the Cairngorms National Park over three years, saw researchers leave out deer carrion for predators at the 'critical' eight-week period when capercaillie are nesting and hatching. The result, as captured on camera traps, was a doubling of the number of broods in areas where alternative food was available, with 85% of monitored capercaillie having chicks in these areas, compared with just 37% in 'unfed' sites. The study team said this equated to an increase in the number of chicks per hen from 0.82 chicks per hen without feeding to 1.90 with feeding – an increase in capercaillie productivity of 130%. (Image: Jack Bamber / St Andrews University) Dr Jack Bamber, from the University of Aberdeen, said 'This study provides compelling, robust, landscape-scale evidence that diversionary feeding can reduce the impact of recovering predators, without killing them, aligning with shifting ethical and ecological goals for conservation management in the UK. 'The combination of rigorous experimentation and innovative monitoring indicates that this method is worth exploration for other species vulnerable to predation, with land managers concerned with other rare prey, and land managers aiming to help capercaillie elsewhere in Europe already considering this tool as an option for them to trial and apply in future.' The team said the study confirmed that the boost in chicks per hen was directly linked to a higher chance that a hen had a brood at all, indicating, they said, that diversionary feeding reduces catastrophic brood failure often caused by nests being raided by predators. READ MORE: Scottish city's economy outperforms London's for first time Diversionary feeding is now endorsed in the Cairngorms Capercaillie Emergency Action Plan, with 18 independent land holdings using the method to help the endangered bird in 2025. Dr Chris Sutherland from the Centre for Research into Ecology and Environmental Modelling at the University of St Andrews said: 'This project is an excellent example of how the impact of research can be maximised when it is co-designed in close collaboration with the wildlife managers and policy makers. 'Doing so enabled us to deliver timely decision-ready evidence underpinned by scientific and statistical rigour'. The findings build on earlier results from an artificial-nest study published in 2024 that found a 50% reduction in pine marten predation, as a result of diversionary feeding, led to a nearly 83% increase in artificial nest survival. The project was a partnership between the University of St Andrews, the University of Aberdeen, Forestry and Land Scotland, RSPB Scotland, NatureScot and Wildland Ltd working under the umbrella of the Cairngorms Connect Predator Project. It was funded by the Scottish Universities Partnership for Environmental Research Doctoral Training Partnership, and the findings were published in the journal Proceedings of the Royal Society B.