
Research into Australian skinks' resistance to snake venom could streamline design of antivenoms
Research led by the University of Queensland has found that multiple species of Australian skink have evolved venom resistance through changes to a critical muscle receptor.
In other animals, the receptor is the target of venom neurotoxins, which cause rapid paralysis and death.
Sign up: AU Breaking News email
Study co-author Prof Bryan Fry, who leads UQ's adaptive biotoxicology lab, said that when venomous snakes arrived in Australia 25 to 30m years ago from Asia that skinks would have been their prey, resulting in evolutionary pressure to evolve venom resistance.
'What was fascinating was that with the kind of mutations that we documented in the Australian … skinks that gave rise to resistance are the kind of mutations that we've seen in other animals outside Australia,' Fry said. Mongooses, which feed on cobras, are one such example.
The researchers showed that the Australian major skink, Bellatorias frerei had the same mutation that gave the honey badger its resistance to cobra venom.
The researchers looked at 47 skink species and found that 13 of these were resistant to snake venom.
Of these 13 species, some had multiple types of resistance, leading the researchers to discover that the skinks had developed independent mutations conferring resistance on 25 occasions.
Fry said the researchers used tissue banks from museums across Australia rather than testing venom on live skinks.
Study co-author and UQ researcher Dr Uthpala Chandrasekara said in a statement: 'We used synthetic peptides and receptor models to mimic what happens when venom enters an animal at the molecular level and the data was crystal clear, some of the modified receptors simply didn't respond at all.'
'It's fascinating to think that one tiny change in a protein can mean the difference between life and death when facing a highly venomous predator.
Sign up to Breaking News Australia
Get the most important news as it breaks
after newsletter promotion
'The more we learn about how venom resistance works in nature, the more tools we have for the design of novel antivenoms.'
Dr Andrew Amey, collection manager of amphibians, reptiles and herpetology at Queensland Museum, who was not involved in the research, said that there were more than 470 currently recognised species of Australian skink, with more being discovered all the time – with little known about them.
'It is great to see research looking into how they deal with such an important predator that just might tell us more about how we can manage the effects of snakebite ourselves,' Amey said.
The study was published in the International Journal of Molecular Sciences.
Hashtags

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles


The Independent
2 hours ago
- The Independent
Why walking further and faster could cut your risk of heart attack and stroke
Walking further and faster could reduce the risk of heart attacks by almost a fifth – even if you don't reach the recommended target of 10,000 steps a day, a study has found. Analysis of more than 36,000 people with high blood pressure revealed walking more reduces the risk of major problems in the heart and blood vessels. The study published in the European Journal of Preventative Cardiology found that compared to a daily step count of 2,300 steps, every extra 1,000 steps was linked to a 17 per cent reduction in the risk of cardiovascular problems. This was the case up to a step count of 10,000 – walking more than this was associated with a lower risk of stroke. It's estimated that just over a quarter adults in the UK, around 14.4 million people, have high blood pressure, according to the British Heart Foundation. It increases the risk of heart disease, heart attacks, strokes and heart failure, according to the NHS. However, study authors say until now it has been unclear how much people with high blood pressure need to increase their physical activity to see a reduction in these risks. Professor Emmanuel Stamatakis, at the University of Sydney, Australia, who supervised the study, said: 'We found that, if you live with high blood pressure, the more you walk with greater intensity, the lower your risk for future serious cardiovascular events. 'These findings support the message that any amount of physical activity is beneficial, even below the widely recommended daily target of 10,000 steps.' Researchers analysed data from 32,192 people with high blood pressure who were part of the UK Biobank study – a cohort study of 500,000 people that collected data on diet, lifestyle, biomarkers, and health. As part of the study these participants agreed to wear an accelerometer on their wrist for seven consecutive days to measure how far and how fast they walked. The participants were followed up for eight years and during this time there were 1,935 cases of heart problems of stroke. After analysing this data researchers discovered for every 1,000 steps a day there was a 17 per cent reduction in overall risk, 22 per cent reduction in heart failure, 9 per cent reduction in risk of heart attack, and 24 per cent reduction in risk of stroke. Researchers also tracked how fast participants walked a day and recorded their fastest 30 minutes at an average of 80 steps a minute which was associated with a 30 per cent reduced risk of heart problems. However, it's not just people with high blood pressure that could benefit. Researchers also found similar results when they looked at 37,350 people without high blood pressure. Every 1,000-step increase in daily step count led to an average lower risk of overall heart problems (20.2 per cent), heart failure (23.2 per cent), myocardial infarctions (17.9 per cent) and stroke (24.6 per cent). Professor Stamatakis said: 'Clinicians should promote physical activity as standard care, especially in patients with high blood pressure. Our results can inform new, tailored public health recommendations for these patients.'


Daily Mail
7 hours ago
- Daily Mail
Birds love to boogie! Cockatoos have 30 distinct dance moves - and even link them together in elaborate routines, study finds
If you thought head banging and body rolls were limited to rock concerts or hip-hop clubs, you'd be mistaken. Experts have discovered that cockatoos have at least 30 different dance moves in their repertoire – including these two signature steps. The routines – of which 17 are newly identified – can even be performed without music. And it shows that the birds' dancing skills are far more common, complex and varied than previously thought. Researchers from Charles Sturt University in Australia and Bristol University in the UK analysed 45 videos posted on social media that showed captive cockatoos dancing. Some of the newly-documented moves include the semi-circle high, the downward head/foot sync, the fluff and the jump turn. The researchers found that some birds also performed their own individual dance moves, often by combining several of the movements in unique ways. The most common move, performed by 50 per cent of birds, was the 'downward' which involved the head bobbing in a downward motion while keeping the eyes facing forward. Next came the 'sidestep', carried out by 43.5 per cent of cockatoos. Other popular moves included the 'fluff' which involves puffing up their feathers, and the headbang. Overall, movements involving just the head were more common while the least common movements were those involving just wings. Next, the researchers investigated dancing behaviour in six cockatoos from three different species housed at Wagga Wagga Zoo in Australia. They played the birds music, an audio podcast, or no audio, and found that all birds performed dance moves, regardless of whether music was being played or not. The study, published in the journal Plos One, revealed that dancing behaviour is present in nearly half of all species of cockatoo. The authors say that further research is needed to determine whether the birds enjoy dancing and whether encouraging this behaviour could improve welfare for captive cockatoos. Professor Rafael Freire said: 'The similarities with human dancing make it hard to argue against well-developed cognitive and emotional processes in parrots, and playing music to parrots may improve their welfare. 'Further research would be beneficial to determine if music can trigger dance in captive birds and serve as a form of environmental enrichment.' Researcher Natasha Lubke added: 'As well as supporting the presence of positive emotions in birds and advancing dance behaviour as an excellent model to study parrot emotions, the work suggests that playing music to parrots may provide a useful approach to enrich their lives in captivity, with positive effects on their welfare.' Several years ago, a viral cockatoo named Snowball was enlisted to help scientists study dancing in animals after becoming a YouTube sensation. The talented bird managed to wow researchers with 14 different moves as he bopped along to 80s classics. The sulphur-crested cockatoo was filmed swinging from side to side, lunging and lifting his foot as he grooved to Another One Bites The Dust and Girls Just Want To Have Fun. Snowball showed a more diverse range of motions when bopping along to Cyndi Lauper's 1983 classic, however. Scientists led by a team from the University of California, San Diego believe his 'remarkably diverse spontaneous movements' show that dancing is not limited to humans but a response to music when certain conditions are present in the brain. British scientists have taught a group of seals how to copy human sounds and one of them how to sing the popular lullaby Twinkle, Twinkle Little Star. Researchers from the University of St Andrews worked with three young grey seals from birth to determine their natural repertoire and say the research could help others study speech disorders. The seals were then trained to copy new sounds, such as vowels and melodies, by changing their formants, the parts of human speech sounds that encode most of the information that we convey to each other. One seal, named Zola, was 'particularly good' at the musical side of things - correctly copying up to 10 notes of songs, including the classic jingle of 'Twinkle, Twinkle'. The study, published in the journal Current Biology, found that the seals used their vocal tract in the same way as us - unlike our closest relatives in the animal world, monkeys and apes. Now scientists working on disorders can use seals as a new model system to study the 'nature vs. nurture' element of our speech development. Researchers Dr Amanda Stansbury and Professor Vincent Janik, of the Scottish Oceans Institute (SOI) at St Andrews worked together on the project. They taught the seals to sing by playing sounds close to their natural vocal range, before rewarding them with treats when they successfully copied them. Dr Stansbury, who now works at El Paso Zoo in Texas, said: 'I was amazed how well the seals copied the model sounds we played to them. 'Copies were not perfect but given that these are not typical seal sounds it is pretty impressive. 'Our study really demonstrates how flexible seal vocalisations are. Previous studies just provided anecdotal evidence for this.' Professor Janik, Director of the SOI, said: 'This study gives us a better understanding of the evolution of vocal learning, a skill that is crucial for human language development.' He added: 'Surprisingly, non-human primates have very limited abilities in this domain. 'Finding other mammals that use their vocal tract in the same way as us to modify sounds informs us on how vocal skills are influenced by genetics and learning and can ultimately help to develop new methods to study speech disorders.' As seals separate from their mothers when they are just two or three weeks old, the findings suggest they could be used to study speech disorders and test different methods for slower learners, the researchers said. 'Since seals use the same neural and anatomical structures as humans to produce these sounds, they provide a good model system in which to study how speech sounds are learned,' Professor Janik said. 'As they separate so early from their mothers, we can control what exactly they hear when, which makes such studies much easier than with humans who are exchanging sounds with parents for all of their development all the time.' A seal called Hoover was documented copying human speech - including phrases like 'how are you?' - at the New England aquarium in Boston, Massachusetts in the 1980s. However, this does not necessarily mean that the mammals could learn to talk like humans. Professor Janik added: 'While seals can copy such sentences, they would not know what they mean. 'We would have to investigate whether they are able to label objects vocally, which is a key requirement for actually talking about things. 'Our study suggests that they have the production skills to produce human language. Whether they can make sense of it would be the next question.'


Times
7 hours ago
- Times
Twinkle-toed cockatoos have 30 different dance moves, researchers find
To most Australians they are mischievous, loud hooligans but researchers have found cockatoos also have an unexpected skill — they are also great dancers. The large, raucous parrots have an impressive 30 different dance moves — including headbanging, sidestepping and body rolls — according to researchers at Australia's Charles Sturt University. They found some individual birds had unique dance moves when hearing music, which often involved combining several movements in new ways. Known for stripping timber, raiding rubbish bins and destroying lawns, cockatoos are among the louts of the bird world but they are also highly intelligent. Not only did the researchers review hours of footage of the birds in the wild and in captivity, they also played the birds music, a podcast or simply left them in silence to see their reactions. Among the songs the cockatoos danced to was The Nights by the Swedish DJ Avicii. The researchers said that they chose 'progressive house' music for the birds to dance too, believing it unlikely the cockatoos had previously heard the songs. All the birds danced, even when there was no music to dance to. The scientists said that the findings showed that at least ten of the 21 known cockatoo species enjoy a boogie, and suggested that playing music to captive birds could help keep them healthy and happy. The study, published in the journal Plos One, said that cockatoo dancing results from complex brain processes including imitation, learning and synchronised, rhythmic movement. Spontaneous dancing in time to music has only been reported in humans and parrots, although some other wild birds also display rhythmic movements as part of their courtship displays, it said. However, what motivates captive birds to dance remains unclear. The researchers analysed 45 videos posted on social media that showed cockatoos dancing. They identified a total of 30 distinct dance movements — 17 of which had not previously been described scientifically. When the researchers then investigated dancing behaviour in cockatoos from three species housed at a New South Wales zoo by playing them music, a podcast or no soundtrack at all, they found that all the birds performed dance moves. Cockatoos, the study said, appeared to display a wide repertoire of dance moves, many of which were similar to the courtship displays of wild parrots. This suggested that their dancing abilities may have originated as courtship behaviour that has been redirected towards their owners when the birds are in captivity. 'The analysis also indicated that dancing is far more complex and varied than previously thought, recording 30 different movements seen in multiple birds and a further 17 movements that were seen in only one bird,' said the lead researcher, Natasha Lubke. 'As well as supporting the presence of positive emotions in birds and advancing dance behaviour as an excellent model to study parrot emotions, the work suggests that playing music to parrots may provide a useful approach to enrich their lives in captivity, with positive effects on their welfare.'