logo
#

Latest news with #neophobia

How to tell if a seagull is going to steal your food
How to tell if a seagull is going to steal your food

The Independent

time25-05-2025

  • Science
  • The Independent

How to tell if a seagull is going to steal your food

A seagull eyeing your lunch or ice cream is unlikely to try to steal it if not part of a flock, scientists have found. Due to an instinct known as neophobia, which means a fear of the unknown, scientists have discovered that herring gulls are mostly reluctant to attack when on their own. But, according to researchers at Ghent University, this fear diminishes when seagulls are part of a flock – potentially putting your food at greater risk. In a study published in the Royal Society Open Science journal, scientists carried out experiments involving a group of 54 seagulls they had reared from hatching. In order to gauge how quickly the gulls would take to approach their food if faced with an unfamiliar object nearby, the scientists placed a range of items – a multicoloured ball, a bucket, brush, folder and jerrycan – next to the gulls' food supply. Some of these objects were familiar to the birds and others were new to them. They found that, when alone, the gulls took an average of 9.81 seconds to approach their food bowl when faced with an unfamiliar object – compared with just 3.52 seconds when in a group. Notably, 24 of the gulls did not eat at all when alone and faced with an unfamiliar object. The scientists also found that the birds were willing to spend more time near their food when in a group than those on their own. Lone gulls spent an average of just 38.8 seconds near their food, compared with 111.8 seconds when in a group – while 16 birds did not approach their food at all. The study states: 'Individuals tested in groups were quicker to eat and spent more time near a novel object than individuals tested alone. 'The results of our study suggest that the presence of group members reduces perceived individual risk, allowing individuals to behave less cautiously.' Noting that 'each bird likely perceives the risk to be shared by the group', the authors said their findings were consistent with previous studies showing that social animals often rely on the presence of the group to make quicker decisions and engage in potentially risky situations. An accompanying release from the Royal Society added: 'When confronted by a gull after your chips it might be best to catch them one-on-one.' While there may be some comfort in the findings that a lone seagull could pose less of a threat of theft, a previous study has suggested that gulls actually prefer food that they have seen being handled by humans, as opposed to food left lying unattended. Although in a further possible line of defence, an earlier study suggested that staring at a seagull could help to deter it from trying to take your food. Scientists at the University of Exeter found that gulls took an average of 21 seconds longer to approach a bag of chips if they could see they were being watched, with just 27 out of 74 birds observed being too cautious to take a chip when a human was nearby.

Gulls not so gutsy when they're alone, Ghent study suggests
Gulls not so gutsy when they're alone, Ghent study suggests

BBC News

time21-05-2025

  • Science
  • BBC News

Gulls not so gutsy when they're alone, Ghent study suggests

Let's set the scene - you sit down to enjoy a delicious snack on a sunny day at the beach when a peckish gull swoops in and swipes your food!It's the stuff of nightmares, right? But the birds might not be as brave as their reputation suggests, according to a new study. The research shows gulls are more likely to take risks - including stealing food - when they're in a group compared to when they're alone. It's down to something called neophobia, which is a fear of things that are new or unfamiliar. As part of the study, a team of researchers from Ghent University in Belgium carried out tests with 54 young herring gulls. They wanted to find out if and how quickly the birds approached food with an unfamiliar object close by, and also whether being around other gulls impacted how quickly they went for the food. Each of the birds was given a unique marking before the test so they could be easily detected by a camera mounted on the roof. Before the testing, a plate filled with fish was placed at the back of an enclosure close to either an object the birds were already familiar with, or one that was brand new to them. Some of the objects included a blue bucket, a colourful ball and a yellow brush. Then either a single bird, or group of gulls was given access to the enclosure for 10 minutes at a time. What were the results? The study found the birds in groups were quicker to eat the food presented to them compared to gulls which were tested alone. More specifically, the birds in groups with familiar objects ate the food the quickest, with the birds in groups with new objects eating the food at the second fastest rate. Individual birds with unfamiliar objects took the longest time to eat, with 24 of them not eating anything during the trial. The final results suggest the presence of fellow gulls reduces the risk the birds feel when they're alone, encouraging them to behave with less caution. So next time you see a group of gulls eyeing up your food, watch out - you might want to make a move before they do!

Why a flock of seagulls will steal your lunch — but a lone gull won't act on its own
Why a flock of seagulls will steal your lunch — but a lone gull won't act on its own

The Independent

time21-05-2025

  • Science
  • The Independent

Why a flock of seagulls will steal your lunch — but a lone gull won't act on its own

A seagull eyeing your lunch or ice cream is unlikely to try to steal it if not part of a flock, scientists have found. Due to an instinct known as neophobia, which means a fear of the unknown, scientists have discovered that herring gulls are mostly reluctant to attack when on their own. But, according to researchers at Ghent University, this fear diminishes when seagulls are part of a flock – potentially putting your food at greater risk. In a study published in the Royal Society Open Science journal, scientists carried out experiments involving a group of 54 seagulls they had reared from hatching. In order to gauge how quickly the gulls would take to approach their food if faced with an unfamiliar object nearby, the scientists placed a range of items – a multicoloured ball, a bucket, brush, folder and jerrycan – next to the gulls' food supply. Some of these objects were familiar to the birds and others were new to them. They found that, when alone, the gulls took an average of 9.81 seconds to approach their food bowl when faced with an unfamiliar object – compared with just 3.52 seconds when in a group. Notably, 24 of the gulls did not eat at all when alone and faced with an unfamiliar object. The scientists also found that the birds were willing to spend more time near their food when in a group than those on their own. Lone gulls spent an average of just 38.8 seconds near their food, compared with 111.8 seconds when in a group – while 16 birds did not approach their food at all. The study states: 'Individuals tested in groups were quicker to eat and spent more time near a novel object than individuals tested alone. 'The results of our study suggest that the presence of group members reduces perceived individual risk, allowing individuals to behave less cautiously.' Noting that 'each bird likely perceives the risk to be shared by the group', the authors said their findings were consistent with previous studies showing that social animals often rely on the presence of the group to make quicker decisions and engage in potentially risky situations. An accompanying release from the Royal Society added: 'When confronted by a gull after your chips it might be best to catch them one-on-one.' While there may be some comfort in the findings that a lone seagull could pose less of a threat of theft, a previous study has suggested that gulls actually prefer food that they have seen being handled by humans, as opposed to food left lying unattended. Although in a further possible line of defence, an earlier study suggested that staring at a seagull could help to deter it from trying to take your food. Scientists at the University of Exeter found that gulls took an average of 21 seconds longer to approach a bag of chips if they could see they were being watched, with just 27 out of 74 birds observed being too cautious to take a chip when a human was nearby.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into the world of global news and events? Download our app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store