logo
Unlock Your Memory by Memorizing Less - Chasing Life with Dr. Sanjay Gupta - Podcast on CNN Audio

Unlock Your Memory by Memorizing Less - Chasing Life with Dr. Sanjay Gupta - Podcast on CNN Audio

CNN4 days ago

Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:00:00
You know, even if you're not a Los Angeles Lakers fan, it is hard not to admire LeBron James. For his amazing athleticism on the court, yes, but also for a more unexpected ability.
Reporter
00:00:11
The start of the fourth, I think they cut it to 14. Do you have any idea what, I mean, I they scored seven quick ones. Anything what happened there?
LeBron James
00:00:20
What happened? We ran them, the first possession, we ran them down all the way to two on the shot clock. Marcus Morris missed a jump shot, followed it up, he got a, they got a dunk. We came back down, we run a set for Jordan Crawford, I mean Jordan Clarkson, and he came off and missed it. They rebounded it. And we came back on the defensive end, and we got a stop, they took it out on the sideline. Jayson Tatum took the ball out, threw it to Marcus Smart in the short corner, he made it three. We come back down, miss another shot, and then Tatum came down and went 94 feet, did a little step, and made a right hand layup timeout.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:01:00
LeBron has an incredible memory. I mean, that is some vivid recollection, sparing no details. And maybe you're left wondering, what's the special sauce that makes his ability to remember so unique?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:01:13
I hope he's listening to this because I'd love to talk to him about his memory, but he has such an exquisite knowledge of the game that he can reduce what's happening around him into one pattern.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:01:26
That's Dr. Charan Ranganath, Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience at the University of California at Davis, where he also directs the Dynamic Memory Lab. Recently, Dr. Ranganth wrote a book. It's called "Why We Remember," and in it, he touched on professional memory athletes. And no, I'm not still talking about LeBron.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:01:46
They're in these competitions where they try to memorize as many digits of pi as they can or try to memorize the order of a deck of cards. And in general, that's a lot of information, right?
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:01:59
'Now if you're someone who can normally only store the highlight reel, but you want to work on remembering the play-by-play, Dr. Ranganath says the key to remembering more may be to memorize less. I'm Dr. Sanjay Gupta, CNN's chief medical correspondent, and this is Chasing Life. The title of the book is "Why We Remember." So why do we remember?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:02:30
I would say that we remember because we need to have the capability to understand what's happening in the now and to anticipate what's about to happen in a pretty uncertain and sometimes scary world. And so if you're packing for a trip, let's say there's some disaster and you have to go report on it as medical correspondent, you're not going to take everything you own, right? You're going to try to say, Hey, what do I need? I might need a jacket. I might need some boots because it's gonna be raining out there And so you'll pack some stuff, you'll go there. You might say to yourself, hey, look, I don't have everything I need. Oh, I wish I brought a razor or something. And you might have packed something that you didn't need, right? Like you packed a sweater and it turned out to be warmer than you thought. But on average, you do pretty well. And that's what memory is like, right. Memory is not about packing every experience that you have for the journey of life. It's really about packing what you need. Based on what your brain thinks is going to be important for the future. And so I think that's the fundamental thing people have to understand about memory is it's not supposed to be complete. It's not suppose to be perfect. It's suppose to give you what you need when you need it. So having a good memory is not remembering everything, but it's remembering better, remembering the things that are important. How accurate is memory? I would say that memory is like a painting. So in other words, you wouldn't look at a painting and go that painting is true or that painting is false. You'd say, well, this painting is a true to the vision of the artist, right? And so, you know, it might have details that are faithful to the thing that they're trying to paint. But at the same time, there's parts that are going to be distorted or inaccurate or missing. And there's also going to some parts of it that just reflect the person's perspective at time, right? For instance, let's say if somebody is dating somebody and then they break up, well, as soon as they break their memories of the relationship are probably going to be negative, either memories of things that their partner did that wrong them or memories of guilt or something. But then 20 years later, that person ends up in a new relationship, they're happily married and just loving life. And now they can look back at that relationship really differently because their present is so different that they can see things from a different perspective. Uh, and that's a big part of what I used to do when I was doing psychotherapy. It was really not about changing necessarily the memories themselves, but people's relationship with their memories and allowing them to paint those memories from a difference perspective.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:05:06
One of the main points that you make is that happiness and satisfaction don't necessarily come from what you've experienced, but more from what you remember. And reading that part of the book, you know, it really sort of struck me that if I imagine something in a certain way, if I reflect on it, let me say, in a certain way I can greatly enhance the memory of that experience.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:05:33
Yes, absolutely. Absolutely. Because there's so many different ways in which we can approach and look at the same memory, right? And this affects how not only how we look at the past and how happy we were with it, but also our ability to plan for things that we'd like to do in the future. So if you look back at a memory of vacation, unless you really sit around and think about, you know, the trouble you had parking in the airport garage and the it took going through security and you know, maybe issues with your baggage, you probably instead be thinking more about how much fun you had with your family or, you know the beautiful beach that you're sitting on, and that's going to make you more likely to take a vacation again. And so Danny Kahneman, the great psychologist who won the Nobel Prize would say, well, this is utterly irrational, right? Um, but I actually think it's not so irrational because those high points are really worth it in some ways. I don't think it gives us necessarily a worse quality of life to be able to look back on those points that you remember as being the ones that are the most important.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:06:38
You're experiencing self versus your remembering self. Danny talks about that as well, right, Professor Kahneman? What is the experiencing self versus the remembering self?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:06:50
Well, we know for a fact that forgetting happens very, very quickly, right? So, um, as much as I would love to think that your listeners are hanging on every word of this conversation, if they remember even 20% of what we've talked about, you know, within a week from now, that'll be a gigantic success, right. And so the experiencing self that we're having is basically one version of ourself. But then when we remember, we've got a very selective piecemeal narrative, as you said, that describes this experience in a meaningful way, but it's very different from what we experienced, right? It's almost like Severance, that TV show where you have like these experiencing selves that are going out and they're the outies, I guess. And then it's like the remembering self is this innie who has only limited access to what happened.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:07:42
By the way, do you like that show?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:07:43
I love that show. It is one of the greatest explorations of the mind I've ever seen in fiction. It's beautiful.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:07:50
Wow. That's saying something coming from you, Charan, I think. But just sticking with this remembering self versus experiencing self, it's interesting because Maya Angelou once said, you know, people may not always remember what you said, but they will remember how you made them feel, right? So this idea, Charan, that you and I are having this conversation, we're talking about memory, obviously, but I'm sitting here looking at you on the screen and you're looking at me and... You've got a nice smile on your face. You present this in a very, very accessible way that I think is joyful. That, I think, makes a difference for me, right? The idea that you're making me feel a certain way about this topic. Is that going to influence how I ultimately think about it?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:08:35
'Absolutely. If you look back, I think most people, if you ask them just to randomly pull out memories of their life, they're not going to typically be the most bland things about like, you know, I remember where I parked my car five days ago. They're going to be typically things that were associated with joy or with anger or with love or desire. And there are actually chemicals in the brain, chemicals I'm sure your listeners have heard about like dopamine, serotonin, or adrenaline that are released during these intensely emotional states. And those actually promote plasticity. They allow so-called consolidation or, you know, really affirming of the memories that happen so that they're much more likely to stick around. And so that's part of why you'll remember how people made you feel. And again, you could say, well, why? Why would we have such a selective memory that's based on these feelings that we have? But I mean, if you think about it, feelings tend to be associated with things that are biologically important, right? It's like if you were a cave person, you walk into a cave and a saber-toothed tiger mauls you or something like that, you should remember that later on. And you should not only remember that it happened, but also how you got there, right. And so there's a reason that memory for traumas stick around because it's in a way our brain trying to protect us from not doing these things again, or try to avoid these threats that are out there in the world, right? Likewise, if we, if something great happens that, you know, you have this great experience with a prospective mate or something like that, that's a really good thing to keep in mind for later on. So our brains are trying to look out for us and these emotional experiences are a big indicator. That a particular experience is important and worth keeping for later on.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:10:27
You know, it's interesting. A lot of people sometimes will be surprised at what they still remember. I still remember from my childhood, like taking a walk with a favorite auntie of mine when I was very young, maybe five or six years old. I still remembered certain things about what the weather was like that day. Why do certain things like that stick? Whereas, you know, studying for an algebra test when I was in high school, that left me much more quickly.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:10:53
You know, you could always try to reverse engineer things that you remember versus things that you don't. And it's really hard because there's so many factors that go into it. But clearly the emotional part of the experience and the attachment that you have both to your auntie, I mean, you said she was your favorite auntie. And that attachment to the place as well are a big part of why you remember it. And it's not necessarily that you remember everything about that experience. I mean, you could probably take an hour of that experience and maybe you have a couple of sentences that you could use to describe it. So you have an experience of that moment you can pull up, but there's not necessarily all the detail that goes with it because you can use your imagination to fill in those blanks.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:11:40
Yeah, I've often wondered, like, how much of these good memories that I have from childhood are actually true, you know? And how much it has been my imagination that have filled in some of those gaps over time. And I guess, does it matter? If memory serves as the story of me, then it is my story, right? So maybe it doesn't matter so much.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:12:03
Absolutely. I would say that it doesn't necessarily matter so much. I mean, I think right now we have a little bit of a problem, which is that people remember what they want to remember.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:12:13
Right.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:12:14
And that could be a little disturbing because we don't want to be living in the matrix or something like that. And I do think we have a real problem in the sense that if you think about our collective memories, that is the memories of things that we've all experienced. Right now there's a real attempt to manipulate collective memory in the sense of like using social media or using other methods to basically reshape people's collective memories of our past and I think this is a real problem and it also can create a problem in terms of implanting memories for information that's just inaccurate that can lead people to become suspicious of science, for instance, and as a medical professional, I'm sure you're aware. And thinking a lot about how problematic the spread of misinformation is, right? So on one hand, we don't want to be too critical of our own autobiographical memories, and at the same time, we have to be critical consumers of information that can seep into our memory.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:13:14
Is memory the problem there or is it the interpretation of memories? Meaning it's raining outside and one person says, this is terrible, it's going to cause flooding and this and the other thing and another person says it's rainy outside and it's gonna help the flowers bloom. Is that what you're talking about or might people actually not remember that it was raining outside?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:13:35
Well, yes, and yes, so let's get into this a little bit. So two people watching the same football game will remember different things that happen based on their beliefs that shape what they take in and how they interpret that information later on and the narratives that they put together. And so those beliefs definitely shape how we construct the past, right? But then on top of it, you have another layer, which is that our memories are fungible and they change, right? So if you and I talk about that same experience later on, as it happens, we can actually remember less collectively after we've talked about it, than we would have if you and I just individually experienced that day and didn't talk about it. And not just that, but we can actually grow to misremember it if you misremember something and you spread that to me, it's sort of like a virus or something. That it can be contagious so that the misinformation now can seep into my memory. So once we share a memory, it's no longer mine or yours. It's ours because of the fungible nature of memory.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:14:44
If it's my team that I'm rooting for whatever, I may remember the more positive aspects of that and willingly or intentionally or not, sort of imprint those on you.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:14:56
'Exactly, exactly. And so for instance, let's just say you believe your team is better than, you know, the other team, but you got screwed because the referee had made some mistakes. Right. I also believe that. And so we start talking about it together and now we build an even more selective memory of what happened. And we forget the times that the quarterback threw an interception or we forget the fumbles that happened in that game. Right. And so Now we've developed a more extremely inaccurate version talking to each other, but there's even another layer to this, which is what I find to be the really scariest part, which is that when we don't have a fully explicit memory of one particular event, our experiences can still change us in the sense that they can make certain concepts seem more familiar to us. Right. And so if you start to hear the same thing from 10 different sources, what happens is it just seems more believable to us because it seems more familiar and lots of research has shown that your ability to think about whether something is true and share that information with other people is based significantly on how much experience you have just hearing it, right? So if you hear from 10, different people that vaccines are bad because they all read the same Badly researched blog post or they heard the same badly researched like podcast. Well Eventually that'll seem more believable Because you've heard it so many times and to the extent that it agrees with your pre-existing belief It'll seem even more believably. So that part of our brains constant tuning itself up that plasticity can be really hijacked in certain ways and lead us to just construct our own realities. And that's what I really worry about right now.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:16:51
Okay, we're going to take a quick break here. When we come back, Dr. Ranganath is going to explain how to best absorb the nearly 12 hours of information we take in daily. It's interesting. I mean, getting back to this Maya Angelou quote, how you feel at the time you're hearing something, if you're attaching, whether it be a concern about vaccine, if you are attaching it to a very emotional story of a child being injured by a vaccine or something like that, that will probably have an outsized impact on people's memory overall. If you're trying to counterbalance that with facts and data and worldwide data. It may not hold as much merit as one emotional story.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:17:37
Yes, absolutely, absolutely. That story that captures your imagination will be definitely something that will weigh more heavily than all the information that you've received that was fairly bland and not emotionally riveting. There's an added part of this too, which is people's, you know, certain learning and your learning of causes and effects. Is more related to things that you've done than things that haven't happened or that you haven't done. So it's just harder intuitively to learn the cause effect relationship between taking a vaccine and not getting sick than it is to learn the relationship between taking your vaccine and having a bad side effect from it.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:18:19
Right, that's interesting, this idea of trying to prove a negative, like you took this and nothing happened to you. You know, I think when people first saw a novel virus coming out, and novel almost being the operative word here, even more than virus, people, I think the instinct is to stick this in a bucket that they know, right? SARS virus, China, oh, I know that bucket. That's 2003. That's SARS from 2003. This is going to behave the same way. Terrible virus but did not expand very much outside of South and Southeast Asia. Or they may think, respiratory virus, this is going to be like flu. You immediately want to stick it into a context bucket that you know. Whereas, if something is novel, you haven't experienced it before. There is no bucket in which to place it. But that's not a comfortable thing, I think, for people. Getting back to the idea of memory. You want to put it in the neighborhood of previous memories, right? You wanna make the unfamiliar familiar in some way.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:19:24
'Yeah, yeah. And in fact, what you can find is that when there is conflict between different possible explanations, what could happen or when there's this uncertainty, I mean, it really causes this discomfort. You can see it in brain activity all over the place, especially in an area called the anterior cingulate. So people have this motivation to resolve that uncertainty. But there's different ways you can go about it, right? One is just pure anxiety and, you know, or another is just to convince yourself that in fact, this is something we've seen before. But a third option is curiosity and we've studied this in our lab. And so you can actually say to yourself, you know, this is a little bit different than anything that we've before. It resembles SARS in some superficial ways, but maybe we should learn more and we should find out more and use that to guide our information seeking. And that's where you can see, for instance, even when people respond to uncertainty with curiosity, we can see activation in these dopamine-carrying areas of the brain and it promotes learning. And so it can be, again, a different narrative that you put onto the same information and stimulate maybe a more productive way of dealing with it.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:20:41
It seems like people are not that comfortable with uncertainty. People are trying to inject certainty into a fundamentally uncertain world. They want to see black and white where they should rightly see gray. That's challenging, and I guess that's where memory starts to fill in gaps, and that seems like where it can get dangerous.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:21:03
And the more that uncertainty relates to distinct threats, the more motivated we have to pull up the right information. But again, our brains also have this balance, which is to say, we're not always going to get it right. And so our brains have this other mechanism that allows us to say I'm going to find new information to supplement what I've already seen before. So we both attend to things that don't match up with what we've seen previously. And we have this capability to then use that to learn new information. I think this is something that I talk about a lot, which is learning's uncomfortable. Learning shouldn't feel easy or good. You know, if a kid's getting straight A's and not even studying for the test, they're not really learning, are they? And when there's uncertainty, those are the opportunities for us to learn the most.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:21:58
I have three teenage girls, and we're going to the whole process of colleges and what classes to take for the next year. And I think you're absolutely right. It's an ongoing negotiation in our household of how to navigate that, I'm sure maybe for you as well. One of the things you wrote in the book is that the average American is exposed to 34 gigabytes, 11.8 hours worth of information every day. I couldn't even get my head around that like that. That's 11.8 hours of information every day, which is far greater than what our ancestors were exposed to.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:22:33
Last time I looked it up, the estimate increased even more since then.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:22:39
How is it that we could be absorbing nearly 12 hours worth of information every day?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:22:43
Well, we're not. I mean, you know.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:22:46
'We're being exposed to it, but we're not actually --
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:22:47
We're exposed to it, but we're not necessarily taking it in. I think one of the kind of misconceptions out there, we can see this in the age of AI, is that we're supposed to be taking everything in that's around us. And in fact, our brains really operate on this principle of economy, to get as little information in as possible and to make as much of that information. And that's why our brains, you know. As smart as you are, your brain is using far less energy than the lighting that's being put on you right now, um, compared to like ChatGPT, which can take down an entire power, right, so it's, it's all about this economy and being able to use attention as this big filter to be able to focus on the things that are most important. And sometimes it's the things that you expect and sometimes it's the stuff that violates your expectations that's where there's the most meaning there but it also means that we miss things sometimes and we end up with frustration because our attention was directed in the wrong place at the wrong time.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:23:48
Attention and intention, that's the answer you write in terms of why you are able to remember certain things and why you forget other things. Memory is an intentional act. And I'm raising my left hand right now, I'm doing that obviously with intention. People think of memory as I'm going through my day, I saw this red cup over here, I saw that those wipes over there, whatever it might be, and they're automatically just going to be imprinted in my memory. But in fact, I have to be intentional about it, right?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:24:19
'Mm-hmm. That's absolutely right because essentially especially now, you know, we live in this world with with Devices and with you know information sources that are designed to grab our attention. And that's why I distinguish between attention and intention because the intention is saying here's my goals. Here's what I what's important to me.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:24:41
Do you also fall prey to this as a cognitive psychologist and neuroscientist? Do you doom scroll social media and do things that are designed to grab your attention away from you?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:24:52
Absolutely, but I think I will say that I've done a lot to reduce it. I've removed ruthlessly alerts on my phone and alerts on my computer, all these things that by default they force you to take in. And if those apps were alerting me to something dumb. Those moments where I break away from our conversation now actually create these artificial boundaries in my head about what was what we were talking about. Then I come back and I have this moment where I have to get back on track and that actually you can see a little like blip of activity in these areas of the brain like the prefrontal cortex when we have to switch back and forth between these different goals and it leaves us essentially one step behind. We have a blurrier memory, also a little bit of stress from having to just keep using that control to get us back on track. So sometimes it's not even what's around you, but rather the habits that we develop that lead to this internal drive to shift away from goals.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:25:54
'I'm sure you get this question all the time. If you're a memory researcher, people think their memory is poor, they're going to come to you and say, how do I improve my memory? And you write about this idea that most humans can keep just a few items in their mind at one time. Uh, even people who are well-trained professional memory athletes, they have the same limitations as everyone else. Mm-hmm. So what What is the strategy then to try and improve your memory if we're all limited by the same thing [and] you can only keep a few things in your mind at once, how do you improve your memory?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:26:32
'Well, the thing that I like to say is don't try to remember more, remember better. And sometimes remembering better means memorizing less. So you brought up the idea of a memory athlete, and these are people who compete in these, well, they're in these competitions where they try to memorize as many digits of pi as they can, or try to the order of a deck of cards. And in general, that's a lot of information, right? So what the person who's a memory athlete does is they develop strategies that allow them to meaningfully slot the information that they're trying to remember into this larger structure so that 10 things can become one thing, right. So I remember when I was trying to learn the lines on the music staff, they would give you this acronym: Every Good Boy Does Fine, right? And then for the open spaces it would be "face," F-A-C-E. And that gives you now instead of like all of these little things to remember E actually I have to run through Well, you know, you can go EFGA, obviously, but, but nonetheless, it gives you these this powerful way of taking all this information and lumping it into one thing, right? And so if you look at people like chess experts, what you find is, is that their knowledge of the game allows them to meaningfully create one pattern or one chunk out of an extremely complex sequence of moves with all of these different pieces on the board, right? And I talk to the book about LeBron James as another wonderful example of this. I hope he's listening to this because it's like, I'd love to talk to him about his memory, but I suspect this is true of many athletes in high speed sports where there's a lot of complexity going on, but he has such an exquisite knowledge of the game that he can reduce what's happening around him into one pattern. And what that allows him to do is not only see and remember what happens at a particular moment of the game, but also to use those patterns to predict what's likely to happen in real time. And so even though these things are happening at a split second pace, because he has that knowledge, he can not only remember these events and you can go on YouTube and find these beautiful examples of him recalling games in, you know, massive detail to the where you could put the video right next to what he's actually saying and be able to see in time exactly how he's remembering it and how good it is, right? And we all have that capability, but again, it requires a little bit of intention to think meaningfully about what's happening now and be able put it together. So, interestingly, like, you know, if you meet someone, you get curious about their life story and you ask them a little bit more about what they do and a fun fact and so forth, it can make it easier to remember their name because now you have a richer set of cues that you can integrate this little piece of new information into.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:29:40
I remember that it's on page 62 of your book, talking about this concept of chunking, right? Chunking all these various things together like you're describing.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:29:49
You really remember page 62? It's not in front of you? Page 62. Oh my goodness, that's amazing. See, this is why you survived med school, and why I would have done terrible.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:29:58
Well, I don't know if you do, but it is interesting, the idea of sticking things in to the memory banks for periods of time for exams and things like that. I mean, I do think a lot of professions sort of rely on that. You think AI is gonna change how important memory is? I mean the way that calculators changed how important it is for us to be able to do long form math in our head. Is AI gonna change how much we need to actually remember?
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:30:26
I would say that it will probably change how we remember and it will change also how memory influences the way we do things, especially in doing things differently than the way that our brains do them in terms of being able to complement our limited attention and our ability to take in large amounts of information. I think also AI has the potential to be the tail that wags the dog in the sense that I always talk about this example of how when I started using Google's AI to complete my phrases. When I write an email, half the time I'll go, nope, that's not what I wanted. Another quarter of the time, I'll say, that's exactly what I want. And then a quarter of time, I'll just say it's not I wanted, but it's good enough, right? And so what happens often is, is that, that good enough then shapes our brain and sort of trains our brain to say good enough is actually good, you know? And so you get these algorithms that feed you songs and these songs are based on lots of songs that you have. And so, you don't explore new songs. You end up developing tastes that become narrower and narrower. It probably closer to the, just the general common denominator, right, because AI itself is based on the lowest common denominator in the sense that it's just scraping everything off the internet in many cases for these large language models. And what makes humans unique is this capability to remember these singular experiences that we have in our life, right? You had a college roommate who was obsessed with Camus and that's given you something that changes the way you think right now. And I think the thing is, is that our experiences can be extraordinarily diverse and give us this beautiful toolbox of random information that, that we've experienced and AI will filter that stuff out typically because it doesn't, it didn't live in your dorm. It didn't know that person who was obsessed with Camus. And so we can use it to compliment our experiences, but it's really important to have these diverse experiences, to read things that aren't recommended to us, and meet people who come from different backgrounds and so forth. And I know diversity has taken on this terribly political kind of view right now, but really I say diversify our training data. We can, like, expose ourselves to sources of information that go beyond AI, because AI is not getting a random sample of population, it's getting kind of a smaller sample that's overpopulated with certain ideas and certain thoughts. And so I think we need to kind of be cognizant of that and force ourselves to get out of our comfort zone in order to be able to get a richer imagination to stay relevant in the age of AI, right?
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:33:20
Charan, what a pleasure to talk to you.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:33:22
Oh, it's amazing to talk to you too. Really, really impressed with all that you've done to spread science to everyone. So thank you for having me on.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:33:32
Oh no, it's my pleasure and thank you for that, and congratulations on the book. I mean, you know, you learn all these things and then you put it out there in the world, and I think we're all better for it. So thank you.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:33:44
Thank you. Couldn't be happier hearing that.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:33:47
Maybe we'll go have dinner in Napa sometime.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:33:49
I would love that.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:33:50
That'd be a nice thing to remember.
Dr. Charan Ranganath
00:33:51
Yeah, or you come to my house, the best Indian restaurant to Davis.
Dr. Sanjay Gupta
00:33:55
Okay, I love it. I love that. Thank you, Charan. Thank you so much. That was my conversation with neuroscientist and memory expert, Dr. Charan Ranganath. He provided us some great insight and tips that I truly hope we all remember. Thanks so much for listening. Chasing Life is a production of CNN Audio. Our podcast is produced by Eryn Mathewson, Jennifer Lai, Grace Walker, Lori Galarreta, Jesse Remedios, Sofía Sanchez, and Kyra Dahring. Andrea Kane is our medical writer. Our senior producer is Dan Bloom. Amanda Sealy is our showrunner. Dan Dzula is our technical director, and the executive producer of CNN Audio is Steve Lickteig. With support from James Andrest, Jon Dionora, Haley Thomas, Alex Manasseri, Robert Mathers, Leni Steinhardt, Nichole Pesaru, and Lisa Namerow. Special thanks to Ben Tinker and Nadia Kounang of CNN Health and Katie Hinman.

Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

Pennsylvania food banks worry about SNAP cuts in federal government's proposed budget bill
Pennsylvania food banks worry about SNAP cuts in federal government's proposed budget bill

CBS News

time30 minutes ago

  • CBS News

Pennsylvania food banks worry about SNAP cuts in federal government's proposed budget bill

Food banks fear that if the budget bill heading to the U.S. Senate gets passed, thousands of people in Pennsylvania will go hungry. The Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program helps nearly 2 million Pennsylvanians put food on the table. "For every meal the food bank provides, SNAP provides nine meals," said Jennifer Miller, CEO of the Westmoreland Food Bank. Leaders from the Westmoreland Food Bank and Feed Pennsylvania came together with the secretaries of the Pennsylvania Departments of Human Services and Agriculture to discuss how proposed federal changes would impact the most vulnerable in the state. They said the House-passed reconciliation bill would cut nearly $300 billion from the SNAP program through 2034. "We have existing work requirements in SNAP, but this bill would make them more strict. And as a result, we believe at least 140,000 Pennsylvanians could lose access to food assistance that helps people be healthy enough to go to work in the first place," Pennsylvania Department of Health and Human Services Secretary Val Arkoosh said. Food banks fear they will see substantially more people lining up for food. "We are not equipped to absorb the massive demand that would result from reduced access to federal nutrition programs. Food banks cannot replace the scale, the reach and the stability of the SNAP program," Miller said. "If enacted, these cuts would eliminate more meals per year distributed by the entire charitable food network in this country," said Julie Bancroft, CEO of Feeding Pennsylvania. State Agriculture Secretary Russell Redding said losing SNAP dollars would also hit farm families. "Roughly 25 cents of every grocery dollar spent goes straight back to the farm, 25 cents for every dollar for food purchased at the grocery store," Redding said. Arkoosh said the proposed cuts would cost the state over $1 billion more annually. "The result would be devastating for Pennsylvania families and for our economy," Arkoosh said. Many believe the fight is not over, though. "You all have a role in contacting your senators, your congressperson, letting them know how this impacts our commnity, our neighbors, our friends," Westmoreland County Commissioner Ted Kopas said.

Versatile Jerar Encarnación returns to Giants after March surgery on his broken left hand
Versatile Jerar Encarnación returns to Giants after March surgery on his broken left hand

Associated Press

time36 minutes ago

  • Associated Press

Versatile Jerar Encarnación returns to Giants after March surgery on his broken left hand

SAN FRANCISCO (AP) — Bob Melvin has been waiting for the chance to write Jerar Encarnación's name into San Francisco's lineup. The versatile Encarnación came off the 60-day injured list Monday and was available for the opener of a four-game series with the San Diego Padres at Oracle Park, though not yet in the starting lineup. Melvin hopes that he could start Tuesday — whether that's at first base or in right field. Encarnación underwent surgery in March on his broken left hand after he was injured trying to make a diving catch during spring training. He batted .302 with two home runs and 14 RBIs in Cactus League play after hitting .248 with five home runs and 19 RBIs in 113 at-bats last year. 'We know he can give us some power and he's got power to all fields,' Melvin said. 'We saw it at the end of last year, we saw it in spring training. When we were about to leave spring training there were going to be a lot of at-bats for him.' The Giants could use a big boost at the plate, and Encarnación hopes to deliver. 'I'm just going to do what I'm able to do to contribute to the team,' said Encarnación, a Dominican Republic native who made his major league debut with Miami in 2022 and joined the Giants as a free agent last May. The Giants optioned outfielder Luis Matos to Triple-A Sacramento so he can further develop and play regularly. San Francisco returned home having dropped five of nine games on its road trip to Washington, Detroit and Miami. The Giants entered Monday having scored only 30 runs over their last 14 games — the club's fewest in such a stretch since being limited to 28 runs from June 20-July 5, 2013. 'That's the good thing about him is he can play multiple positions, he can pinch hit,' Melvin said. 'It's nice to have him back. Spring training we were talking about how impactful he was going to be. He was having a great spring and next thing you know he's out for a while. He feels good at the plate, he hit some home runs the last couple days, he's ready to go.' Encarnación has been eager to rejoin the Giants, but embraced his faith and that it took the time it did for him to fully recover and come back. He missed the first 59 games, then made seven rehab appearances with Triple-A Sacramento last week, playing three games at first base, starting two as designated hitter and two more in right field. 'I'm so happy and content that I'm here,' he said, before adding with a smile a few minutes later that he's 'great, muy bueno.' ___ AP MLB:

Former DC teacher arrested, charged with soliciting child pornography: DOJ
Former DC teacher arrested, charged with soliciting child pornography: DOJ

Fox News

time41 minutes ago

  • Fox News

Former DC teacher arrested, charged with soliciting child pornography: DOJ

A former Washington, D.C., schoolteacher has been arrested by the FBI for allegedly soliciting child pornography from a minor, the Justice Department said Monday. Devonne Keith Brown, 56, who taught health at the IDEA Public Charter School, is accused of soliciting child sexual abuse materials from a young girl in Prince George's County, Maryland. "Those who prey on minors to solicit the production of CSAM are morally reprehensible and will be held accountable for their heinous crimes," said Steven Jensen, assistant director in charge of the FBI Washington Field Office. "The FBI remains committed to protecting our children and ridding our communities of this grievous scourge." Brown is charged with one count of receipt of child pornography and appeared in court Monday. He was detained by a judge pending a Thursday hearing. In addition to soliciting child porn, Brown also allegedly used CashApp to send payments to the minor, federal prosecutors said. "for snacks ? more photos otw," one comment states on a $15 CashApp request sent to Brown from a minor, according to court documents. The unidentified minor told investigators that she met a man named "Joseph" at a grocery store near her home. She said the man sent her money for ice cream via the app. After communicating with the man on social media, she sent him nude images of herself, authorities said. The man who went by "Joseph" also sent the minor images of nude women and asked her to recreate them for him, prosecutors said. Upon further investigation, authorities allege that they also found sexually graphic images and videos in emails sent to Brown by minors. In a statement to FOX 5 DC, the charter school said it was "deeply concerned" by the allegations against Brown. "This individual is no longer employed by the school," the school said. "The safety and well-being of our students is our highest priority. We are fully cooperating with law enforcement and supporting their ongoing investigation. Based on the information currently available to us, there is no indication that any IDEA student was directly involved." Fox News Digital has reached out to the school.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into the world of global news and events? Download our app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store