logo
Do chimpanzees have rhythm? Listen to their drumming.

Do chimpanzees have rhythm? Listen to their drumming.

Yahoo09-05-2025

'You feel it in your body when they drum—it's insanely impressive,' says Vesta Eleuteri.
She's a cognitive biologist at the University of Vienna who studies chimpanzee drumming and observed over 350 drumming performances like this in five African countries.
Chimpanzees in West Africa hold onto trees with their hands while kicking against roots with their feet, emitting powerful sounds that travel long distances, sometimes over a half mile.
'They're so fast sometimes… their hands and feet are a blur,' says Catherine Hobaiter, primatologist at the University of St. Andrews, who was also involved with the research. 'They will jump between the buttresses on these huge rainforest trees and almost dance around them, hitting them with their feet and hands. It's really amazing to see.'
Eleuteri and Hobaiter's findings that chimpanzees drum rhythmically on tree roots, and that different groups drum with different styles, was recently published in Current Biology. Their observations suggest drumming is a way to share information with other members of the group. It's a unique mode of communication that humans and chimpanzees share. Scientists think it may have arisen in a common ancestor the two species share.
Another new study on some of the same chimpanzees, published in Science Advances, found they combine vocal sounds to convey complex meanings, a communication system which could be a stepping stone between animal communication and human language.
Together, these chimpanzee behaviors can help scientists understand how human of music-making and language first developed.
'You've really got the building blocks of the kind of things we've used to make modern music, present long before humans were humans,' says Hobaiter.
Rhythmic music-making has long been considered uniquely human, and percussion is one of the earliest forms of human musical expression. Drumming can have different individual and regional styles but typically consists of non-random timing: hits are evenly spaced, aligning to a set rhythm.
Eleuteri and Hobaiter's previous research discovered chimpanzees drum on buttress roots—large, wide roots growing above the soi—to send information over long and short distances. Each chimpanzee has their own unique drumming style, and they combine drumming with hooting to share where they are in the forest and what they're doing.
Although all animals communicate, humans are considered the only species on Earth known to use language: combining sounds into words, and words into sentences, to create meaning.
Other animals like birds vocalize to send messages, and while some species do combine these calls, this behavior is typically limited to a few types of calls and to the context of alerting others to predators. Bonobos, which are closely related to chimpanzees and humans, also use complex combinations, primatologists think the mysterious origins of human language might come from a common ancestor shared by all three species 5 to 7 million years ago.
The new drumming study analyzed 371 drumming bouts in 11 chimpanzee communities in Côte d'Ivoire, Guinea, Senegal, Uganda, and Tanzania.
'Until recently, there wasn't a lot of convincing evidence for rhythmic behavior in primates that has any resemblance with human musical rhythm,' Pritty Patel-Grosz, a linguist at the University of Oslo who was not involved in the study but whose research explores rhythmic dancing in gibbons, wrote in an email. '[The] observation that chimpanzee drumming on tree buttresses exhibits properties of human musical behavior is another huge breakthrough.'
The same research project studying drumming chimps in Côte d'Ivoire also contributed to the new study on how chimps combine vocalizations. It analyzed over 4,000 recorded sounds from 53 chimpanzees.
Calls were connected to specific events: feeding, nesting, approaching, aggression, or predator encounters. Two-call vocalizations were combined to convey more meaning than a simpler call, spanning a range of day-to-day activities: for example, a 'hoo" sound was usually used during feeding and travel, and a 'pant' sound was mostly used during social activities with other chimpanzees, but a combined 'hoo' and 'pant' sound was used during nesting, creating a whole new meaning.
'We expected the system to be versatile, but that was beyond our expectations,' says study author Cédric Girard-Buttoz, evolutionary biologist at the French National Center for Scientific Research (CNRS). 'The system has most of the linguistic phenomena we find in language.'
Complex chimpanzee vocalizations could represent an evolutionary bridge between more simplistic animal calls and open-ended human language, says Girard-Buttoz.
'Chimpanzee communication may incorporate the seeds of a human-like system,' Patel-Grosz writes of the findings.
'In human cognition, many scholars have been arguing that language and music are closely related, and they may share the same cognitive resources,' wrote Patel-Grosz. For example studies showing the same parts of the brain process language and music.
Patel-Grosz proposes further research can better clarify this connection: 'In other words, did chimpanzee vocal communication and chimpanzee rhythmic drumming evolve together, and can this inform our understanding of the evolution of language and music?'
'When investigating the evolution of human language, we usually compare it to the vocal communication of [animals],' says Maël Leroux, evolutionary biologist at University of Rennes, who was not involved in the new studies. 'Human language is not vocal, it's very much multimodal,' a combination of sounds and gestures.
That suggests the origins of human language are also multimodal, Leroux says.
Different drumming styles were observed in the different communities researchers studied, which may also reflect the variation in human language, says Leroux.
Western chimpanzees typically drummed more often and faster, while eastern chimpanzees drum more slowly and start drumming later in their hooting. Hobaiter thinks these differences are shaped by the different social structures seen between the eastern and western groups—eastern chimpanzees are more aggressive and hierarchical, and western chimpanzees are more cohesive and egalitarian—and Hobaiter points out that since rhythm is central to human sociality, dance, and speech, our social behaviors may have been tied to how we developed rhythm too.
'This is the kind of science that wakes us up to the fact that every single population of chimpanzees is worth conserving and preserving,' says Hobaiter. 'We're starting to recognize that they might have cultures in their communication, in their rhythm, in their social behavior… if you lose a group, you lose the unique culture that goes with it.'

Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

Quantum system beats classical AI in real test, powers greener supercomputing future
Quantum system beats classical AI in real test, powers greener supercomputing future

Yahoo

time7 hours ago

  • Yahoo

Quantum system beats classical AI in real test, powers greener supercomputing future

In a major leap toward the future of computing, researchers have shown that even small-scale quantum processors can outperform classical algorithms in machine learning tasks. The finding offers a glimpse into a faster, greener era within the relatively new research field of Quantum Machine Learning, a space gaining momentum across both academia and new study combines quantum computing and machine learning, two of the most disruptive technologies of our time. Recent advances in both fields are reshaping the technological frontier. While AI is already embedded in everything from personal assistants to scientific research, quantum computing promises a fundamentally new way of processing information. Their intersection has given rise to a rapidly growing field: quantum machine learning. This emerging discipline explores whether quantum systems can improve the speed, accuracy, or efficiency of machine learning algorithms. However, proving such an advantage on today's limited quantum hardware remains a major challenge—one that researchers are just beginning to by an international team led by the University of Vienna, the experiment used a photonic quantum processor to classify data points, an essential task in modern AI systems. The researchers found that the quantum system outperformed its classical counterpart, making fewer errors—a rare, real-world glimpse of quantum advantage with current hardware. This breakthrough was achieved using a quantum photonic circuit developed at Italy's Politecnico di Milano and a machine learning algorithm proposed by UK-based Quantinuum. The experiment marks one of the first demonstrations of quantum enhancement in practical AI tasks, rather than simulations. By isolating the quantum contribution in the classification process, the team was able to pinpoint specific scenarios where quantum systems results not only validate the potential of photonic quantum processors but also lay the groundwork for identifying machine learning tasks where quantum computing can make a real-world impact, even with today's limited-scale hardware.'We found that for specific tasks, our algorithm commits fewer errors than its classical counterpart,' said Philip Walther, project lead from the University of Vienna. "This implies that existing quantum computers can show good performances without necessarily going beyond the state-of-the-art technology," adds Zhenghao Yin, first author of the accuracy, the experiment also reveals another important advantage in energy quantum systems process information using light and therefore consume significantly less power than traditional hardware, which is becoming increasingly important as AI's energy demands continue to rise.'This could prove crucial in the future, given that machine learning algorithms are becoming infeasible due to high energy demands,' said co-author Iris showing that today's quantum devices can already offer tangible improvements, the findings could steer both quantum computing and classical machine learning into a more symbiotic future, where quantum-inspired algorithms push conventional boundaries and photonic platforms help make AI more sustainable. The study has been published in the journal Nature Photonics.

Oxford University to host global climate and human rights summit launch event
Oxford University to host global climate and human rights summit launch event

Yahoo

time2 days ago

  • Yahoo

Oxford University to host global climate and human rights summit launch event

A panel of speakers from global climate leadership has been announced for a launch event in Oxford ahead of a climate summit. The University of Oxford will host the launch of the Right Here, Right Now Global Climate Summit 2025 at the Sheldonian Theatre on Wednesday, June 4. Featuring keynote speeches, panel discussions, and performances, the event will bring together leading voices from policy, activism, business, and academia to explore the relationship between climate change and human rights. The launch event will take place ahead of a 24-hour global online plenary extending into World Environment Day on June 5. The evening will begin with introductory remarks from Professor Irene Tracey, vice-chancellor of the University of Oxford, and Volker Türk, United Nations High Commissioner for Human Rights. Professor Irene Tracey, vice-chancellor of the University of Oxford (Image: Contributed) The event will be hosted by Justin Rowlatt, climate editor at the BBC, and will include a rotation of speakers discussing how climate change impacts human rights and how these rights can inform responses to the climate crisis. The panel of speakers will include Lord Alok Sharma, a former COP26 president known for leading international negotiations resulting in the Glasgow Climate Pact. Vanessa Nakate, a climate justice activist and founder of the Rise Up Movement, who advocates for equitable climate solutions focusing on African communities, will also speak. Kate Raworth, an economist and co-founder of the Doughnut Economics Action Lab, whose work explores sustainable development within planetary boundaries, and Paul Polman, a business leader and former CEO of Unilever, known for championing responsible corporate climate action, will speak too. The audience will also hear from Professor Lavanya Rajamani, professor of international environmental law at Oxford, whose research and legal work supports climate-vulnerable nations in international negotiations, Emma Pinchbeck, chief executive of the Climate Change Committee, who leads the UK's independent advisory body on reaching net zero, and Dr Omnia El Omrani, a medical doctor and climate-health policy advocate, who has represented youth voices at recent UN climate summits and focuses on the health impacts of climate change. The event will be livestreamed and will conclude with a handover to partners at the University of the South Pacific in Fiji to begin the global programme at 10pm. The 24-hour plenary will include contributions from university hubs around the world, partners at UN Human Rights, and the International Universities Climate Alliance. The Oxford Mail and Oxford City Council are looking to find Oxford's climate heroes with this year's Oxford Climate Awards. Rose Hill and Iffley Low Carbon at the Oxford Climate Awards 2024 (Image: Joe Baker) They are looking for individuals or groups who champion environmental sustainability across categories including innovation in climate action, sustainable business, and green transport.

Paleontologists dig through fossilized dino guts to see what's inside
Paleontologists dig through fossilized dino guts to see what's inside

Yahoo

time2 days ago

  • Yahoo

Paleontologists dig through fossilized dino guts to see what's inside

Nothing quite fits the moniker 'gentle giant' more than sauropods. These gargantuan dinosaurs could reach up to 123 feet long and weigh up to seven tons. Sauropods have long been believed to be herbivores, munching on leaves during the Jurassic and Cretaceous periods. Now, for the first time, a team of paleontologists have studied the abdomen of a sauropod with its gut contents still intact that lived roughly 94 to 101 million years ago. The finding confirms that they were in fact herbivores–and did not really chew their food. Instead, sauropods relied on gut microbes to break down its food. The findings are detailed in a study published June 9 in the Cell Press journal Current Biology. 'No genuine sauropod gut contents had ever been found anywhere before, despite sauropods being known from fossils found on every continent and despite the group being known to span at least 130 million years of time,' Stephen Poropat, a study co-author and paleontologist at Curtin University in Australia, said in a statement. 'This finding confirms several hypotheses about the sauropod diet that had been made based on studies of their anatomy and comparisons with modern-day animals.' Fossilized dinosaur bones can only tell us so much about these extinct animals. Paleontologists can use trackways and footprints to learn about their movement and preserved gut contents called cololites to put together what their diets may have looked like. Understanding the diet is critical for understanding their biology and the role they played in ancient ecosystems, but very few dinosaur fossils have been found with cololites. These are gut contents that have yet to become poop–or coprolites. In particular, sauropod cololites have remained elusive. With their gargantuan sizes, these dinosaurs may have been the most ecologically impactful terrestrial herbivores on the planet during the Jurassic and Cretaceous periods. With this lack of direct dietary evidence, the specifics of sauropod herbivory—including the plants that they ate—have mostly been theorized based largely on tooth wear, jaw shape and size, and neck length. But that changed in the summer of 2017. Staff and volunteers at the Australian Age of Dinosaurs Museum of Natural History were excavating a relatively complete subadult sauropod skeleton. This particular Diamantinasaurus matildae specimen lived during the mid-Cretaceous period and was uncovered in the Winton Formation of Queensland, Australia. The team noticed an unusual, fractured rock layer that appeared to contain the sauropod's cololite with well-preserved plant fossils. The team analyzed the plant specimens within the cololite and found that sauropods likely only engaged in minimal oral processing of their food. Instead of chewing, their gut microbiota would ferment the plants to digest it. The cololite had a wide variety of plants, including foliage from conifers (cone-bearing seed plants), seed-fern fruiting bodies (plant structures that hold seeds), and leaves from angiosperms (flowering plants). From this, it looks like Diamantinasaurus was an indiscriminate, bulk feeder. 'The plants within show evidence of having been severed, possibly bitten, but have not been chewed, supporting the hypothesis of bulk feeding in sauropods,' said Poropat. The team also found chemical biomarkers of both angiosperms and gymnosperms—a group of woody, seed-producing plants that include conifers. [ Related: The mystery of why some dinosaurs got so enormous. ] 'This implies that at least some sauropods were not selective feeders, instead eating whatever plants they could reach and safely process,' Poropat said. 'These findings largely corroborate past ideas regarding the enormous influence that sauropods must have had on ecosystems worldwide during the Mesozoic Era.' Although it was not unexpected that the gut contents provided support for sauropod herbivory and bulk feeding, Poropat was surprised to find angiosperms in the dinosaur's gut. 'Angiosperms became approximately as diverse as conifers in Australia around 100 to 95 million years ago, when this sauropod was alive,' he says. 'This suggests that sauropods had successfully adapted to eat flowering plants within 40 million years of the first evidence of the presence of these plants in the fossil record.' Based on these findings, the team suggests that Diamantinasaurus likely fed on both low- and high-growing plants, at least before adulthood. As hatchlings, sauropods would have only been able to access food that was close to the ground. As they grew (and grew and grew), their viable food options also expanded. Additionally, the prevalence of small shoots, bracts, and seed pods in the cololite implies that subadult Diamantinasaurus likely targeted new growth portions of conifers and seed ferns. These portions of the plant are easier to digest. According to the authors, the strategy of indiscriminate bulk feeding likely served sauropods well for 130 million years. However, as with most studies, there are some important caveats and limitations. 'The primary limitation of this study is that the sauropod gut contents we describe constitute a single data point,' Poropat explained. 'These gut contents only tell us about the last meal or several meals of a single subadult sauropod individual.' We also don't know how the seasons affected diet, or if the plants preserved in this specific sauropod represent a diet typical of a healthy sauropod or a more stressed one. The specimen is also a subabult, which could mean that younger sauropods had this more than adults did. Despite the limitations, it offers an exciting look inside the stomachs of some of the largest creatures to ever live.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into the world of global news and events? Download our app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store