logo
Our Genes Reveal Mysterious Split in Human Population 1.5 Million Years Ago

Our Genes Reveal Mysterious Split in Human Population 1.5 Million Years Ago

Yahoo20-03-2025
We've long assumed our species evolved from a tidy, single stream of ancestors. But life on Earth is never quite so straightforward, especially not when it comes to the most socially complex species we know: humans.
University of Cambridge researchers have now uncovered an estrangement in our family tree, which began with a population separation 1.5 million years ago and a reconciliation just 300,000 years ago.
What's more, according to their analysis of modern human DNA, one of these isolated populations left a stronger legacy in our genes than the other.
"The question of where we come from is one that has fascinated humans for centuries," says geneticist Trevor Cousins, first author of the published study.
In biology, we often describe genetics and evolution with the metaphor of a branching tree. Each species' lineage begins with a 'trunk' at the base that represents a common ancestor, shared by all species at the crown.
As we trace the tree from base to tip, which represents evolutionary time, its trunk forks, again and again, each split representing an irreconcilable rift in populations that meant they could no longer breed with each other, and thus became separate species.
What an evolutionary tree does not capture is the on-again/off-again nature of intra-species dynamics, the many near-misses where one breeding group diverges into two, and then blends again back to one.
In some situations, this makes quite a mess of the neat and tidy tree diagram, and calls into question where the precise 'species' cutoff is.
"Interbreeding and genetic exchange have likely played a major role in the emergence of new species repeatedly across the animal kingdom," Cousins says.
Cousins and his co-authors, Cambridge geneticists Aylwyn Scally and Richard Durbin, had a hunch this kind of family drama would apply to our own species, Homo sapiens, which is technically more like a subspecies, except that there aren't any other groups left.
Aside from humanity's general penchant for love and war, there's some proof we 'spliced branches' with the Denisovans, and with a fair bit of Neanderthal DNA in our gene pool to this day, we know species lines must have blurred there, too.
The team used a statistical model based on the likelihood of certain genes originating in a common ancestor without selection events intruding. This was then applied to real human genetic data from the 1000 Genomes Project and the Human Genome Diversity Project.
A deep-rooted population structure emerged, suggesting modern humans, Homo sapiens, are the result of a population that split in two about 1.5 million years ago, and then, only 300,000 years ago, merged back into one. And it explains the data better than unstructured models, the norm for these kinds of studies.
"Immediately after the two ancestral populations split, we see a severe bottleneck in one of them – suggesting it shrank to a very small size before slowly growing over a period of one million years," says Scally.
"This population would later contribute about 80 percent of the genetic material of modern humans, and also seems to have been the ancestral population from which Neanderthals and Denisovans diverged."
It suggests the human lineage became irrevocably tangled much earlier than we thought. For instance, Neanderthal genes are only present in non-African modern human DNA, making up about 2 percent. The ancient mixing event 300,000 years ago resulted in only about 20 percent of modern human genes coming from the minority population.
"However, some of the genes from the population which contributed a minority of our genetic material, particularly those related to brain function and neural processing, may have played a crucial role in human evolution," Cousins says.
"What's becoming clear is that the idea of species evolving in clean, distinct lineages is too simplistic."
The research was published in Nature Genetics.
DNA From Beethoven's Hair Reveals Surprise Nearly 200 Years Later
Crucial Feature of Human Language Emerged More Than 135,000 Years Ago
Mysterious Twist Revealed in Saga of Human-Neanderthal Hybrid Child
Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

2.65 million-year-old fossil teeth unearthed in Ethiopia opens up current understandings of human evolution
2.65 million-year-old fossil teeth unearthed in Ethiopia opens up current understandings of human evolution

New York Post

time10 hours ago

  • New York Post

2.65 million-year-old fossil teeth unearthed in Ethiopia opens up current understandings of human evolution

Scientists in Ethiopia unearthed pieces of 2.65 million-year-old fossilized teeth belonging to two members of a newly discovered Homo species that could challenge previously accepted understandings of human evolution. The team of scientists found a total of 10 teeth — six molars, two incisors, one premolar and one canine — they believe came from two members of the new Australopithecus species, which hasn't yet been named. Scientists found 10 teeth they determined belonged to a new Australopithecus species. via REUTERS Advertisement The new species marks the seventh discovered genus of Australopithecus, an early ancestor of today's people, primarily ape-like in appearance but with human characteristics like bipedal walking. Australopithecus also had dental features almost identical to modern-day people, including a complete set of 32 teeth. The team also unearthed three other teeth that are roughly 2.59 million years old with traits consistent among the oldest-known Homo species first identified in 2013, which hasn't been formally named either. Both species haven't been assigned official names because the collected fossils are still incomplete. Advertisement Still, the fossils provide much-needed insight into a relatively murky era of human evolution, as the close age of the teeth indicates the two unnamed species could've coexisted — or even competed for resources. The teeth also confirm that there were at least four types of hominins throughout East Africa at the time, with a fifth roaming in southern Africa. 'This reinforces the idea that the story of human evolution is not of a single lineage changing slowly through time,' Brian Villmoare, University of Nevada, Las Vegas paleoanthropologist and lead author of the research published on Wednesday in the journal Nature, said. The team also found more teeth linked to a separate Homo species first identified in 2013. via REUTERS Advertisement 'Rather, the pattern of human evolution is similar to that of other organisms, repeatedly branching into multiple species throughout the fossil record, many of whom lived at the same time.' Researchers plan on analyzing the fossils to see if the unnamed species shared similar foods or had any notable interactions. Humans, or Homo Sapiens, are the youngest member of the Homo genus, having only originated roughly 300,000 years ago in Africa. Advertisement The Homo genus is generally believed to have descended from a species of Australopithecus before it eventually died out. The exact species of Australopithecus, though, is hotly contested. With Post wires

Study links fries to higher type 2 diabetes risk
Study links fries to higher type 2 diabetes risk

Yahoo

time4 days ago

  • Yahoo

Study links fries to higher type 2 diabetes risk

A new study has found that eating three or more servings of French fries a week could increase the risk of developing type 2 diabetes. An international team of researchers, including an expert from the University of Cambridge, investigated links between potato consumption and the risk of type 2 diabetes. Overall they found a 20% increased risk for those who ate French fries at least three times a week, but not for those who ate baked, boiled or mashed potatoes. Dr Faye Riley, from Diabetes UK, said: "This research shows that the link between potatoes and type 2 diabetes isn't as clear-cut as it might seem." Academics analysed data from studies tracking the health of more than 205,000 medical workers in the US. Repeated surveys were taken about people's diets over almost four decades and during follow-up periods 22,000 cases of type 2 diabetes were documented. "The association between higher potato intake and increased [type 2 diabetes] risk is primarily driven by intake of French fries," the study authors wrote in The British Medical Journal. "Higher intake of French fries, but not combined baked, boiled, or mashed potatoes, was associated with a higher risk of [type 2 diabetes]." The research team also found replacing three servings of potatoes each week with whole grains was found to lower the risk of type 2 diabetes by 8%. They added replacing potatoes with white rice, however was also associated with an increased risk. Dr Riley, research communications lead at Diabetes UK, said: "Type 2 diabetes is a complex condition, with many factors influencing its development, including genetics, age and ethnicity. "Diet is just one part of the picture, but this study suggests that how food is prepared can make a difference and reinforces the advice to prioritise whole grains and cut back on fried or heavily processed foods as a way to support a balanced diet and reduce your risk." Follow Cambridgeshire news on BBC Sounds, Facebook, Instagram and X.

Students Find Hidden Fibonacci Sequence in Classic Probability Puzzle
Students Find Hidden Fibonacci Sequence in Classic Probability Puzzle

Scientific American

time07-08-2025

  • Scientific American

Students Find Hidden Fibonacci Sequence in Classic Probability Puzzle

A variation of a puzzle called the 'pick-up sticks problem' asks the following question: If I have some number of sticks with random lengths between 0 and 1, what are the chances that no three of those sticks can form a triangle? It turns out the answer to this quandary has an unexpected parallel to a pattern found across nature. The Fibonacci sequence is an ordered collection of numbers in which each term is equal to the previous two added together. It goes like this: 1, 1, 2, 3, 5, 8, 13,..., and so on. These numbers show up practically everywhere. If you look at a plant with spirals, such as a pine cone or pineapple, more likely than not, the number of spirals going in each direction will be consecutive terms of the Fibonacci sequence. But a pair of young researchers were surprised to find that this pattern and the pick-up sticks problem are deeply connected. On supporting science journalism If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing. By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today. The pick-up sticks problem is a variant of the 'broken stick problem,' which can be traced back to at least 1854. In its simplest iteration, the broken stick problem asks the likelihood that a stick broken randomly into three pieces can form a triangle. (In the pick-up stick problem, the lengths don't need to add up to a particular whole, so the possible lengths are distributed differently.) More than a century later, in the October 1959 issue of Scientific American, Martin Gardner wrote about the broken stick problem for his Mathematical Games column. Gardner highlighted it as a classic example of the counterintuitive nature of problems in probability and statistics. In a preprint paper posted to the server in May, the young researchers and their collaborators explore a new variation of the pick-up sticks problem. This effort started when Arthur Sun, a first-year undergraduate student at the University of Cambridge, thought up a problem for a university math contest. What is the likelihood, he wondered, that out of four sticks with random lengths between 0 and 1, no three could make a triangle? He enlisted the help of his friend Edward Wang, at the time a 12th grader at Scotch College, a secondary school in Australia, where he and Sun originally met. Together, Wang and Sun modeled the problem on their computers and ran random trials over and over, keeping track of the results of each trial. It seemed to the pair that four sticks could not make a triangle among them very close to one sixth of the time. Soon Wang and Sun started wondering what the answer was for larger groupings of sticks. They enlisted the help of David Treeby, a mathematician affiliated with Australia's Monash University and a teacher at Scotch College. The group ran even more simulations, and soon a pattern started to emerge. According to the researchers' simulations, if n was the number of sticks selected randomly, the chance of not having a valid triangle among them was the reciprocal of the first n Fibonacci numbers multiplied together. For instance, if you pick six sticks randomly, the probability that you cannot make a triangle with them is 1 / (1 × 1 × 2 × 3 × 5 × 8) = 1 ⁄ 240. The team was surprised that the famous sequence was connected to the triangle problem. 'We'd no reason to suspect that it would be,' Treeby says, 'but it was impossible that it wasn't.' The researchers began to develop a proof of why this connection must be true, but they needed an expert in statistics to pull it all together. They brought in a fourth collaborator, former Monash mathematician Aidan Sudbury. He'd been happily enjoying his retirement when the team approached him. 'I immediately was struck by what a charming problem it was,' he says. 'Delightful!' Together, the four researchers worked out a solid proof of the pattern that Sun and Wang had noticed. Though related results have been proved using similar methods and encompassing a wide array of stick-and-triangle problems, some experts in the field find this new paper's simplicity refreshing. 'What's nice about this is: it's very well written,' says Steven Miller, a mathematician at Williams College and president of the Fibonacci Association. 'It's accessible, it's easy to read, and it's extending a very famous problem.' To understand the pick-up sticks solution, think about the smallest possible case. Suppose you have three sticks with random sizes between 0 and 1. Any three sticks can form a triangle if, and only if, no stick is longer than the other two put together. If you have sticks of lengths 1, 2 and 300, no matter how wide an angle you put between them, the first two sticks could never stretch wide enough to accommodate the third. This is called 'the triangle inequality': if a, b and c represent the lengths of the sticks from shortest to longest, they will only fail to form a triangle when a + b ≤ c. To find the probability that three random lengths form a triangle, mathematicians can consider every set of three lengths as a point in three-dimensional space (for instance, lengths 1 ⁄ 2, 1 ⁄ 6 and 1 ⁄ 3 are represented by the point [ 1 ⁄ 2, 1 ⁄ 6, 1 ⁄ 3 ]). Because the lengths fall between 0 and 1, the set of all such points can be represented by a unit cube: Researchers then look at the subset of this cube where the points satisfy the triangle inequality—a shape that looks like this: With a little geometry, it turns out that this shape is exactly half the volume of the cube. Thus, three randomly picked lengths will be able to form a triangle exactly half of the time, as 1 / (1 × 1 × 2) = 1 ⁄ 2. Where does Fibonacci come in? Suppose a collection of any number of sticks is ordered from shortest to longest. If no three among them form a triangle, each stick's length must be greater than or equal to the sum of the previous two—otherwise, those three sticks could make a triangle. In the Fibonacci sequence, each number is precisely equal to the sum of the previous two. In other words, each segment of the Fibonacci sequence is as close as possible to having a triangle in it without actually having one. In Treeby's words, 'If we [avoid triangles] greedily, the Fibonacci sequence appears naturally.' The researchers feel there should be some path directly from this insight to a proof of the pick-up sticks theorem. They couldn't find one, however. 'We sort of hoped to find something that was a little bit more ... intuitive, but we couldn't formalize our thinking,' Treeby says. Instead their paper uses integrals to calculate the high-dimensional volumes directly—a method a bit like looking at the area inside the cube above (but without the visual reference). The researchers aren't on the prowl for a different proof right now—but they hope someone else might find one.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into a world of global content with local flavor? Download Daily8 app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store