logo
How did iguanas get to Fiji? Chances are, they floated nearly 5,000 miles on vegetation rafts

How did iguanas get to Fiji? Chances are, they floated nearly 5,000 miles on vegetation rafts

CNN21-03-2025
Summary
Ancient iguanas rafted nearly 5,000 miles across the Pacific Ocean from North America to Fiji, research suggests.
The journey, which occurred about 34 million years ago, may be the longest overwater voyage by a land vertebrate.
Genetic evidence reveals Fijian iguanas diverged from their North American desert iguana ancestors roughly when the Fijian archipelago formed.
As cold-blooded animals, iguanas can survive extreme conditions including heat, starvation and dehydration during long journeys.
Scientists believe studying these dispersal events could explain how other species colonize isolated areas across the globe. Around 34 million years ago, the ancestors of modern iguanas likely embarked on what may be the longest overwater journey undertaken by a nonhuman, land-dwelling vertebrate species.
Starting off the epic trek from the western coast of North America, these iguanas traveled nearly 5,000 miles (8,000 kilometers) — one-fifth of the Earth's circumference — across the Pacific Ocean, eventually arriving in Fiji, according to a new study.
Using genetic evidence, researchers propose that these iguanas made the extraordinary voyage by rafting on floating vegetation, possibly composed of uprooted trees or plants.
For decades, scientists have debated how Fiji's iguanas arrived. Previous theories suggested that an extinct species of iguana rafted from the Americas without a clear timeline, while others proposed that the lizards migrated overland from Asia or Australia, said lead study author Dr. Simon Scarpetta, an assistant professor at the University of San Francisco. Scarpetta conducted this research during his National Science Foundation postdoctoral fellowship at the University of California, Berkeley, and in his current role.
The findings, published Monday in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, help clarify the long-standing mystery of how these reptiles reached such remote islands.
Scarpetta and his team aimed to test both the overwater rafting and overland theories, as well as other hypotheses for the biogeographic origin of Fijian iguanas, including dispersal through Antarctica or across the Bering land bridge.
Understanding this type of water dispersal could offer new insights into how other species have colonized isolated areas over time, Scarpetta added.
Tracing genetics
Iguanas have already demonstrated an ability to survive long-distance ocean travel, according to an October 1998 study. At least 15 green iguanas appeared on the beaches of Anguilla in the Caribbean in 1995 on rafts of uprooted trees. Researchers determined the lizards likely floated nearly 200 miles (322 kilometers) from Guadeloupe following a hurricane event.
Scarpetta noted that this type of overwater rafting is often described as 'sweepstakes' dispersal, a rare event that allows a species to colonize an otherwise unreachable area. Major weather events, such as hurricanes or floods, can dislodge vegetation and carry animals along with it.
To determine when iguanas arrived in Fiji, researchers analyzed the genes of 14 living iguana species. The team found that the closest living relative of Fijian iguanas is the Dipsosaurus — a type of desert iguana native to the southwestern United States and northwestern Mexico, according to the new study.
Fossil evidence further supports the idea that these iguanas originated in North America, as no fossils of desert iguanas have been found elsewhere in the world, Scarpetta said.
The analysis also suggests that the Fijian iguanas diverged from their American ancestors between 34 million and 30 million years ago, settling around the same time as the volcanic formation of the Fijian archipelago, Scarpetta said.
This timeline challenges previous theories that iguanas may have made a complex overland journey from South America via Antarctica, which would have happened much later in history, said study coauthor Dr. Jimmy McGuire, a professor of integrative biology at University of California, Berkeley.
'In phylogenetic analyses there is always some degree of uncertainty when trying to predict the timing of divergence events between species,' said Dr. Shane Campbell-Staton, an associate professor of ecology and evolutionary biology at Princeton University, who was not involved in the study. 'In this case, the authors were very thorough in collecting multiple different kinds of genetic data and use(d) multiple different models … to test their hypothesis and find that most of the results are largely in agreement.'
How was the trek possible?
While a monthslong voyage across the Pacific may seem impossible, iguanas are surprisingly well-equipped for such extreme travel.
'If you had to pick a vertebrate group that could survive a rafting event across thousands of kilometers of open ocean, iguanas are a great choice,' Scarpetta said.
Many iguana species, especially those in desert environments, can endure extreme heat, starvation and dehydration, Scarpetta noted.
'Being ectothermic means that you don't expend many of your food resources or fat reserves maintaining an elevated body temperature,' McGuire said, referring to the metabolic rate of cold-blooded animals. 'Ectotherms can be about 25 times more efficient in this regard than endotherms (warm-blooded animals), so they don't need to eat nearly as much food nor nearly so frequently.'
Some estimates suggest that an overwater journey from North America to Fiji could have taken anywhere from four to 12 months. However, newer simulations indicate the trip may have been closer to 2 ½ to four months, Scarpetta said.
Despite basic survival challenges, food scarcity likely wouldn't have been a major one. If necessary, the herbivorous reptiles may have been able to feed on their floating vegetation mats, McGuire said.
Scientists said they hope that by studying dispersal events, they may be able to predict which species can survive long-distance overwater travel, offering new insights into how animals spread across the globe.
'We now know that overwater dispersal is not only possible, but it has likely played a large role in shaping species diversity on islands around the world,' Campbell-Staton said via email.
Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

Two Thirds Of Trash Found In Global Rivers Is Plastic, Study Finds
Two Thirds Of Trash Found In Global Rivers Is Plastic, Study Finds

Forbes

timea day ago

  • Forbes

Two Thirds Of Trash Found In Global Rivers Is Plastic, Study Finds

Pieces of plastic, including bottles, bags, straws and cutlery, make up two thirds of the rubbish found in rivers around the world, according to a new analysis. The research by experts at the University of California, Santa Barbara, used on-the-ground data from Mexico, Jamaica, Panama, Ecuador, Kenya, Vietnam, Thailand and Indonesia. Over the three-year period, local teams collectively removed and analyzed 3.8 million kilograms of river debris, the equivalent to 380,000,000 single-use plastic water bottles, with 66% classified as microplastic. The researchers also found substantial variation in the amount of plastic pollution intercepted in rivers studied, but all contained plastic debris of some sort. Based on their findings, researchers now estimate 1.95 million metric tons of travels down rivers worldwide every year. The study has been published as negotiators in Geneva failed to reach an agreement on an international treaty to curb plastic pollution. Lead author, Chase Brewster, a project scientist in University of California, Santa Barbara's Benioff Ocean Science Laboratory, said it was largest continuous dataset, which has ever been produced on the subject of plastic pollution in rivers, in an interview. Brewster added the research helps underline and scope and the scale of the issue around the world. 'This problem is not going away and it is global,' he told me. 'We should not be ignoring in this day and age the fact two thirds of everything coming down our rivers is now plastic.' Brewster said the researchers on the project also engaged with local communities and he hoped the data would help them take targeted action 'The more involved and engaged people are, and the more empowered they feel in their communities, the more likely they are to change behavior and influence legislation in their areas. 'More often than not, these communities are not trying to pollute. It's the larger institutions who are failing them. There's no access to waste management services, or other options.' Brewster said the report makes a number of recommendations, including creating more of a value for plastic waste to discourage people from throwing it away and letting it go to waste. He added one of the big problems is that it is still cheaper to produce virgin plastic than it is to make recycled plastic, so there need to be incentives like bottle deposit fees, and virgin production caps to create better market conditions for a circular economy. The study also calls for greater support for the informal waste-picking sector in certain parts of the world and more investment in waste management and recycling infrastructure and services. It also recommends more consistent and more transparent monitoring and data collection to inform targeted upstream actions. In addition, it calls for well-designed local and national policies as well as ambitious international frameworks to address the different scales of the problem. 'Negotiators and country officials can use this data to assess the river plastic pollution problem in real life, look at differences in pollution in different places, and test ideas about what policies and systems are working,' Brewster added. Anthony Merante, senior plastics campaigner at Oceana Canada, said the oceans have become the dumping grounds for the world's plastic waste, in an email. Merante added people have over-plasticized their lives and invested in single-use, disposable systems hoping that recycling will save us. 'Not a single river studied was free from plastic, emphasizing that this is a planetary problem we all must act to correct,' he said. 'The solution is clear; we need to reduce plastic production and use and invest in solutions that actually work like refill and reuse.'

Photos: Inside the Allen Institute for AI's new HQ in Seattle's first mass-timber office building
Photos: Inside the Allen Institute for AI's new HQ in Seattle's first mass-timber office building

Geek Wire

time2 days ago

  • Geek Wire

Photos: Inside the Allen Institute for AI's new HQ in Seattle's first mass-timber office building

The view from one of Ai2's outdoor patios, overlooking Lake Union and the Seattle skyline. (Photos by Todd Bishop, GeekWire) It was a big news week for Seattle's Allen Institute for Artificial Intelligence (Ai2), including the announcement of a new AI robotics initiative and a landmark grant from Nvidia and the National Science Foundation to lead the creation of the future AI backbone for U.S. scientific research. But the nonprofit research institute also reached another milestone in recent weeks: moving into its new 50,000-square-foot headquarters in Seattle's first large-scale mass-timber commercial building. A lobby seating area at Ai2's new headquarters in Northlake Commons. (GeekWire Photo / Todd Bishop) The new Ai2 space, in the Northlake Commons project on the north shore of Lake Union, is now the central gathering and workspace for the organization and its team of about 225 people. Ai2 occupies one floor of the five-story building. It's a short drive up the road from Ai2's prior headquarters, and within walking or biking distance of the University of Washington, where many of Ai2's researchers are also affiliated with the UW Allen School of Computer Science & Engineering. Mass timber is an engineered wood product made by binding layers of wood together into strong, large structural panels and beams, providing the strength of steel or concrete with a lower carbon footprint. The mass-timber elements are visible throughout the space and common areas of the building. An outdoor meeting area at the Northlake Commons building. The space includes large meeting rooms, private call rooms, a podcast and video studio, large doors that open up to outside patios on warm days, and a dining area and gathering space with sweeping views of Lake Union and downtown. The interior design deliberately avoids a traditional 'sea of desks' layout in favor of smaller groupings of workstations where researchers can collaborate while remaining connected through walkways and sight lines. There's even enough space for a simulated home environment in the Ai2 robotics lab, including two kitchens, allowing researchers to test their latest AI robotics technology in conditions that mimic a real-world household. The Ai2 logo in the lobby of the new headquarters. Northlake Commons was designed by Seattle-based architect Weber Thompson and built by general contractor Swinerton. News of the Ai2 lease was announced in July 2024. The Allen Institute for AI (Ai2) is separate from the AI2 Incubator, which spun off from the non-profit and has its own new waterfront home, AI House, at Pier 70 on Elliott Bay. Keep scrolling for more pictures from inside the new Ai2 space. The view across the open-air courtyard at Northlake Commons, the mass-timber building that now houses Ai2's new headquarters. (GeekWire Photos / Todd Bishop) A grouping of workstations inside the new Ai2 headquarters. The entrance to a new podcast and video studio at the Ai2 headquarters. A large common area overlooking Lake Union. The Ai2 logo inside the common room of the new headquarters. RELATED STORY: Allen Institute for AI lands $152M from Nvidia and NSF to lead national AI project

Scientists Identify a New Glitch in Human Thinking
Scientists Identify a New Glitch in Human Thinking

Gizmodo

time2 days ago

  • Gizmodo

Scientists Identify a New Glitch in Human Thinking

Good news, everyone! Scientists at the University of California, Berkeley, have coined a new term to describe our brains being dumb. In a recent study, they provide evidence for a distinct but common kind of cognitive bias—one that makes us reluctant to take the easier path in life if it means retracing our steps. The researchers have named the bias the 'doubling-back aversion.' In several experiments, they found that people often refuse to choose a more efficient solution or route if it requires them to double back on the progress already made. The findings suggest that people's subjective fear of adding more to their workload and their hesitance to wipe the slate clean contribute to this bias, the researchers say. 'Participants' aversion to feeling their past efforts were a waste encouraged them to pursue less efficient means,' they wrote in their paper, published this May in Psychological Science. The 12 cognitive biases that prevent you from being rational Psychologists have detailed all sorts of biases related to digging our feet in when faced with important new information. People tend to stick to the status quo in choosing dinner at a favorite restaurant, for example, even when someone recommends a potentially tastier option. There's also the sunk cost fallacy, or the reluctance to veer off a disastrous path and choose another simply because they've spent so much time or resources pursuing it. The researchers argue that their newly named bias is certainly a close cousin to the sunk cost fallacy and similar biases, but that it ultimately describes a unique type of cognitive pitfall. In their paper, they provide the example of someone whose flight from San Francisco to New York becomes massively delayed early on, leaving them stuck in Los Angeles. In one scenario, the traveler can get home three hours earlier than their current itinerary if they accept the airline's offer of a new flight that first stops in Denver; in the second, the person is instead offered a flight that will also shave three hours off, but they'll first have to travel back to San Francisco. Despite both flights saving the same amount of time, people are more likely to refuse the one that requires going back to their earlier destination, the paper explains (some people might even refuse the Denver flight, but that would be an example of the status quo bias and/or sunk cost fallacy at work, they note). To test their hypothesis, the researchers ran four different types of experiments. The experiments collectively involved more than 2,500 adults, some of whom were UC Berkeley students and others who were volunteers recruited through Amazon's Mechanical Turk. In one test, people were asked to walk along different paths in virtual reality; another asked people to recite as many words starting with the same letter as possible. Across the various tests, the researchers found that people routinely exhibited this aversion. This Is What Your Brain Looks Like When You Solve a Problem In one experiment where people had to recite words starting with 'G,' for instance, everyone was asked midway through if they wanted to stay with the same letter or switch to reciting words starting with 'T' (a likely easier letter). In the control condition, this decision was framed as staying on the same task, simply with a new letter, but in the other, people were asked if they wanted to throw out the work they had done so far and start over on a new task. Importantly, the volunteers were also given progress bars for the task, allowing them to see they would perform the same amount of work no matter the choice (though again, 'T' would be easier). About 75% of participants made the choice to switch in the control condition, but only 25% did the same when the switch was presented as needing to double back. 'When I was analyzing these results, I was like, 'Oh, is there a mistake? How can there be such a big difference?'' said lead author Kristine Cho, a behavioral marketing PhD student at UC Berkely's Haas School of Business, in a statement to the Association for Psychological Science, publishers of the study. Other researchers will have to confirm the team's findings, of course. And there are still plenty of questions to answer about this aversion, including how often we fall for it and whether it's more likely to happen in some scenarios than others. But for now, it's oddly comforting to know that there's another thing I can possibly blame for my occasional stubbornness to take the faster subway train home.

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into a world of global content with local flavor? Download Daily8 app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store