
Whale said: How far have we come in the quest to eavesdrop on animals?
The buzzing of bees, calls of birds, lion's roar and dolphin's screech were just a background score in this production in which we were the main characters; nobody else got any lines.
Then, in the early 1900s, Karl von Frisch proved that honeybees perform a dance in the hive that conveys to other bees where the nearest food sources are. This kicked off the study of how animals communicate.
By the 1960s, it was clear that certain species call and sing to each other in notes that vary with the message: summons, warning, invitation to mate. One such set of sounds, recorded by Roger Payne, Katy Payne and Scott McVay, became the best-selling 1970 album Songs of the Humpback Whale.
Now, researchers are finally beginning to decode how some of this communication works.
In February, for instance, an international study published in the journal Science analysed eight years of recordings from humpback whales to find that certain shorter sounds are used far more frequently than other, more complex ones. Essentially, whale songs fall into a pattern called the Zipfian distribution, in which the most common word in a language (eg 'the') shows up twice as often as the second-most-used word, and three times as often as the third-most-common word (and so on).
The Zipfian distribution is common to all human languages. The fact that it appears to hold true for whales indicated that their songs convey far more than innate, reflexive responses to external stimuli.
The unexpected discovery was made because the study roped in behavioural ecologists, scientists and linguists to work with the marine biologists. This kind of collaborative research is part of what is pushing the envelope in this field; the other big change, of course, is machine learning (ML) and artificial intelligence (AI).
Last June, ML helped a team of researchers (led by behavioural ecologist Mickey Pardo of Colorado State University) analyse 469 vocalisations made by family groups of adult female African elephants in Kenya and their offspring, over 14 months. Their study, published in Nature Ecology & Evolution, showed that the animals appear to have unique calls for specific individuals.
When these calls were played back to the elephants, the researchers noted, specific individuals reacted to their 'names', perking up their ears and rumbling back in reply.
In the Dominican Islands, meanwhile, since May last year, marine biologist David Gruber and his research organisation Project CETI (for Cetacean Translation Initiative) have been working to use AI to decode what sperm whales are saying, in their Morse-code-like pattern of clicks called coda. They plan to then use AI to generate clicks of their own, in an effort to 'speak back'.
Elsewhere, marine biologist Laela Sayigh and her team at the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution studied the wide range of clicks and whistles used by wild bottlenose dolphins off the coast of Florida and believe they have deciphered how two of the 22 types of whistles are used. One functions as an alarm and the other as a query, says a study published as a preprint on bioRxiv this April. 'Our study provides the first evidence in dolphins for a wider repertoire of shared, context-specific signals, which could form the basis for a language-like communication system,' Sayigh and her team note.
In May, building on these signals, Google DeepMind released DolphinGemma, a large language model (LLM) that aims to help researchers gather and analyse 'dolphin-speak'.
Does this mean we could gain the ability to communicate, at least in limited ways, with certain species?
Well, two fundamental hurdles would remain, even if we did decode every whale click and dolphin whistle. Most animals appear to communicate mainly to signal ('food', 'danger', 'it's time to mate', etc), which is a more limited version of the skill humans have evolved. Meanwhile, many animals use visual, chemical and mechanical signals, alongside sound, in ways that humans do not. This could potentially make any 'speaking back' on our part both difficult and meaningless.
What we can expect, for now, is to at least gain a greater understanding of how meaningful, layered and even deeply intentional animal communication can be. Like an elephant calling out across the grasslands in Kenya, and her calf turning around, recognising the distinct sound his family uses just for him.
Hashtags

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles


Hindustan Times
13 hours ago
- Hindustan Times
Whale said: How far have we come in the quest to eavesdrop on animals?
For centuries, human beings believed they were the only species that could 'speak'. MAMA?: Research has shown that elephants respond to the unique calls family groups use for specific individuals. (Adobe stock) The buzzing of bees, calls of birds, lion's roar and dolphin's screech were just a background score in this production in which we were the main characters; nobody else got any lines. Then, in the early 1900s, Karl von Frisch proved that honeybees perform a dance in the hive that conveys to other bees where the nearest food sources are. This kicked off the study of how animals communicate. By the 1960s, it was clear that certain species call and sing to each other in notes that vary with the message: summons, warning, invitation to mate. One such set of sounds, recorded by Roger Payne, Katy Payne and Scott McVay, became the best-selling 1970 album Songs of the Humpback Whale. Now, researchers are finally beginning to decode how some of this communication works. In February, for instance, an international study published in the journal Science analysed eight years of recordings from humpback whales to find that certain shorter sounds are used far more frequently than other, more complex ones. Essentially, whale songs fall into a pattern called the Zipfian distribution, in which the most common word in a language (eg 'the') shows up twice as often as the second-most-used word, and three times as often as the third-most-common word (and so on). The Zipfian distribution is common to all human languages. The fact that it appears to hold true for whales indicated that their songs convey far more than innate, reflexive responses to external stimuli. The unexpected discovery was made because the study roped in behavioural ecologists, scientists and linguists to work with the marine biologists. This kind of collaborative research is part of what is pushing the envelope in this field; the other big change, of course, is machine learning (ML) and artificial intelligence (AI). Last June, ML helped a team of researchers (led by behavioural ecologist Mickey Pardo of Colorado State University) analyse 469 vocalisations made by family groups of adult female African elephants in Kenya and their offspring, over 14 months. Their study, published in Nature Ecology & Evolution, showed that the animals appear to have unique calls for specific individuals. When these calls were played back to the elephants, the researchers noted, specific individuals reacted to their 'names', perking up their ears and rumbling back in reply. In the Dominican Islands, meanwhile, since May last year, marine biologist David Gruber and his research organisation Project CETI (for Cetacean Translation Initiative) have been working to use AI to decode what sperm whales are saying, in their Morse-code-like pattern of clicks called coda. They plan to then use AI to generate clicks of their own, in an effort to 'speak back'. Elsewhere, marine biologist Laela Sayigh and her team at the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution studied the wide range of clicks and whistles used by wild bottlenose dolphins off the coast of Florida and believe they have deciphered how two of the 22 types of whistles are used. One functions as an alarm and the other as a query, says a study published as a preprint on bioRxiv this April. 'Our study provides the first evidence in dolphins for a wider repertoire of shared, context-specific signals, which could form the basis for a language-like communication system,' Sayigh and her team note. In May, building on these signals, Google DeepMind released DolphinGemma, a large language model (LLM) that aims to help researchers gather and analyse 'dolphin-speak'. Does this mean we could gain the ability to communicate, at least in limited ways, with certain species? Well, two fundamental hurdles would remain, even if we did decode every whale click and dolphin whistle. Most animals appear to communicate mainly to signal ('food', 'danger', 'it's time to mate', etc), which is a more limited version of the skill humans have evolved. Meanwhile, many animals use visual, chemical and mechanical signals, alongside sound, in ways that humans do not. This could potentially make any 'speaking back' on our part both difficult and meaningless. What we can expect, for now, is to at least gain a greater understanding of how meaningful, layered and even deeply intentional animal communication can be. Like an elephant calling out across the grasslands in Kenya, and her calf turning around, recognising the distinct sound his family uses just for him.


News18
3 days ago
- News18
Experts Warn Public To Avoid Rabbits With Dangerous Head Growths In Colorado
US experts have been alarmed by the sighting of the 'Frankenstein' bunnies with dangerous black head-spikes caused by a rapidly spreading virus. 'Frankenstein' rabbits with horrifying head spikes have been sighted in the United States of America. According to a report by the New York Post, these bunnies feature a terrible-looking set of spikes emerging out of their face. The report also claimed these black and dangerous spikes in cottontail rabbits are caused by a rapidly spreading virus. These Frankenbunnies with tentacle-like growths out of their heads were spotted by the locals in Fort Collins, Colo. One of the residents, Susan Mansfield, told 9News that she saw a rabbit with what appeared to her as 'black quills or black toothpicks sticking out all around his or her mouth." Mansfield confirmed she had seen the rabbit the previous year, thinking it would've died by now. 'I thought he would die off during the winter, but he didn't. He came back a second year, and it grew," the woman said. Another local described the infected cottontail rabbits as carrying 'a scabby-looking growth over their face." Experts Alarmed By 'Frankenstein' Rabbits With Dangerous Black Spikes The head spikes observed by these locals are indeed a sign of cottontail papilloma virus, which is also known as Shope papilloma virus that results in sprout tumours emerging around the head of the cottontail bunny. According to Pet MD, the virus spreads through parasites like ticks and fleas, passing on the ailment by biting the little bunnies. 'Typically rabbits become infected in the warmer months of summer when transmitted by being bitten by insects like fleas and ticks," said Colorado Parks and Wildlife spokesperson Kara Van Hoose, as quoted by The Coloradoan. Colorado may be the recent site of these bunnies with horn-like growths, but the disease is understood to have originated in the Midwest. In 2013, a Minnesota resident raised alarm bells among locals by posting a video of a tendril-ridden rabbit in his backyard, later calling it the 'Frankenstein' bunny. Wildlife Experts Warn Against Interacting With Bunnies Carrying Spikes US experts have been alerted by the sighting of the 'Frankenstein' bunny and have warned locals not to approach or handle any of the afflicted critters. In their report, Colorado Parks and Wildlife have clarified, however, that the cottontail papilloma virus can spread only between rabbits and is not found to infect humans or pets, which would provide relief to the concerned public. But the little cottontail rabbits are not as lucky. While there may be instances of the virus leaving these bunnies in time, in some cases, the Medusa-like protrusions can swell and interfere with the infected carrot eater's ability to consume food, causing starvation and death. view comments First Published: August 14, 2025, 12:03 IST Disclaimer: Comments reflect users' views, not News18's. Please keep discussions respectful and constructive. Abusive, defamatory, or illegal comments will be removed. News18 may disable any comment at its discretion. By posting, you agree to our Terms of Use and Privacy Policy.


Time of India
3 days ago
- Time of India
Meet Chrysalis: The spaceship that could be humanity's one-way ticket to a 400-year-journey to space!
Humanity has always looked to the stars with curiosity, hope, and a constant thought, what if one day we could journey beyond our solar system? Now, engineers have brought that dream a little closer by proposing a bold vision for interstellar travel, through a rotating spacecraft designed to ferry thousands on a centuries‑long voyage to the nearest star. While it remains purely conceptual, Chrysalis ignites our imagination and brings us face-to-face with serious questions like, What would it take to build a self‑sustaining civilization in deep space? How would future generations live, learn, and preserve their humanity as they move between the stars? Which spacecraft is proposed to carry people to space Project concept, Photo: Chrysalis Via Project Hyperion Design Competition Engineers have proposed an extraordinary spacecraft named Chrysalis, envisioned to carry up to 2,400 people on a one‑way, multi‑generational journey to Alpha Centauri, the closest star system to our Sun. The vessel spans 58 km in length, arranged in a concentric 'nesting‑doll' architecture with rotating sections to create artificial gravity. According to Project Hyperion's official brief, launched as part of the Project Hyperion Design Competition, Chrysalis took first place for its system‑level coherence and imaginative yet cohesive design. Its internal structure comprises layers dedicated to different functions, including a central zone for food production, farms with tropical and boreal forests, fungi, livestock, microbes, and insects. Apart from that, there will also be surrounding communal spaces such as parks, schools, libraries, and hospitals, residential shells with 3D‑printed modules, and the outermost zones serving as warehouses and industrial areas. The most attractive feature of the spaceship The 'Cosmos Dome', a microgravity viewing area showing transparent panels where people can move across weightlessly and take in the universe beyond, is an intentional design to encourage psychological well‑being. How will Chrysalis work Chrysalis would rely on nuclear fusion reactors, a technology not yet which has yet been developed, to power life support, machining, and habitat systems. The proposed journey spans roughly 400 years, meaning generations will live and die aboard before arriving, likely at Proxima Centauri b, a potential Earth‑like exoplanet. Design and living conditions of the project (Photo: Chrysalis Via Project Hyperion Design Competition) The design is proposed to maintain a stable population of about 1,500 individuals over time, despite the total capacity being 2,400. Social resilience would be supported by artificial intelligence, helping in the resilience of the whole social system, better knowledge transfer between the different generations of inhabitants, and a deeper vision of the overall dynamics of the Chrysalis spaceship complex,' according to the project brief. Preparations for the initial crew would include 70–80 years of adaptation in isolated environments like Antarctica, to ensure psychological readiness for prolonged confinement in space. While this is entirely theoretical, given our current inability to build practical fusion reactors or multi kilometer structures in space, the Chrysalis concept is an exploration into what our far-future outer‑space ambitions might look like.