
What is 'cognitive shuffling' and does it really help you get to sleep?
If you've been on social media lately - perhaps scrolling in the middle of the night, when you know you shouldn't but you just can't sleep - you might have seen those videos promoting a get-to-sleep technique called "cognitive shuffling".
The idea, proponents say, is to engage your mind with random ideas and images via a special formula:
It's popular on Instagram and TikTok, but does "cognitive shuffling" have any basis in science?
The cognitive shuffling technique was made famous by Canada-based researcher Luc P. Beaudoin more than a decade ago, when he published a paper about how what he called "serial diverse imagining" could help with sleep.
One of Beaudoin's hypothetical examples involved a woman thinking of the word "blanket", then thinking bicycle (and imagining a bicycle), buying (imagining buying shoes), banana (visualising a banana tree) and so on.
Soon, Beaudoin writes, she moves onto the letter L, thinking about her friend Larry, the word "like" (imagining her son hugging his dog). She soon transitions to the letter A, thinking of the word "Amsterdam":
"and she might very vaguely imagine the large hand of a sailor gesturing for another order of fries in an Amsterdam pub while a rancid accordion plays in the background."
Sleep soon ensues. The goal, according to Beaudoin, is to think briefly about:
"a neutral or pleasant target and frequently [switch] to unrelated targets (normally every 5-15 seconds)."
Don't try to relate one word with another or find a link between the words; resist the mind's natural tendency toward sense-making.
While the research into this technique is still in its infancy, the idea is grounded in science. That's because we know from other research good sleepers tend to have different kinds of thoughts in bed to bad sleepers.
People with insomnia are more focused on worries, problems, or noises in the environment, and are often preoccupied with not sleeping.
Good sleepers, on the other hand, typically have dream-like, hallucinatory, less ordered thoughts before nodding off.
Cognitive shuffling attempts to mimic the thinking patterns of good sleepers by simulating the dream-like and random thought patterns they generally have before drifting off to sleep.
In particular, Beaudoin's research describes two types of sleep-related thoughts: insomnolent (or anti-sleep) and pro-somnolent (sleep-promoting) thoughts.
Insomnolent thoughts include things such as worrying, planning, rehearsing, and ruminating on perceived problems or failings.
Pro-somnolent thoughts on the other hand involve thoughts that can help you fall asleep, such as dream-like imagery or having a calm, relaxed state of mind.
Cognitive shuffling aims to distract from or interfere with insomnolent thought. It offers a calm, neutral path for your racing mind, and can reduce the stress associated with not sleeping.
Cognitive shuffling also helps tell your brain you are ready for sleep.
In fact, the process of "shuffling" between different thoughts is similar to the way your brain naturally drifts off to sleep. During the transition to sleep, brain activity slows. Your brain starts to generate disconnected images and fleeting scenes, known as hypnagogic hallucinations, without a conscious effort to make sense of them.
By mimicking these scattered, disconnected, and random thought patterns, cognitive shuffling may help you transition from wakefulness to sleep.
And the preliminary research into this is promising. Beaudoin and his team have found serial diverse imagining helps to lower arousal before sleep, improve sleep quality and reduce the effort involved in falling asleep.
However, with only a small number of research studies, more work is needed here.
As with every new strategy, however, practise makes perfect. Don't be disheartened if you don't see an improvement straight away; these things take time.
Stay consistent and be kind to yourself.
And what works for some won't work for others. Different people benefit from different types of strategies depending on how they relate to and experience stress or stressful thoughts.
Other strategies to help create the right conditions for sleep include:
If, despite all your best efforts, night time thoughts continue to impact your sleep or overall wellbeing, consider seeking professional help from your doctor or a trained sleep specialist.
Melinda Jackson, Associate Professor at Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University and Eleni Kavaliotis, Research Fellow in the Sleep, Cognition, and Mood Laboratory at Monash University, Monash University
This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.
If you've been on social media lately - perhaps scrolling in the middle of the night, when you know you shouldn't but you just can't sleep - you might have seen those videos promoting a get-to-sleep technique called "cognitive shuffling".
The idea, proponents say, is to engage your mind with random ideas and images via a special formula:
It's popular on Instagram and TikTok, but does "cognitive shuffling" have any basis in science?
The cognitive shuffling technique was made famous by Canada-based researcher Luc P. Beaudoin more than a decade ago, when he published a paper about how what he called "serial diverse imagining" could help with sleep.
One of Beaudoin's hypothetical examples involved a woman thinking of the word "blanket", then thinking bicycle (and imagining a bicycle), buying (imagining buying shoes), banana (visualising a banana tree) and so on.
Soon, Beaudoin writes, she moves onto the letter L, thinking about her friend Larry, the word "like" (imagining her son hugging his dog). She soon transitions to the letter A, thinking of the word "Amsterdam":
"and she might very vaguely imagine the large hand of a sailor gesturing for another order of fries in an Amsterdam pub while a rancid accordion plays in the background."
Sleep soon ensues. The goal, according to Beaudoin, is to think briefly about:
"a neutral or pleasant target and frequently [switch] to unrelated targets (normally every 5-15 seconds)."
Don't try to relate one word with another or find a link between the words; resist the mind's natural tendency toward sense-making.
While the research into this technique is still in its infancy, the idea is grounded in science. That's because we know from other research good sleepers tend to have different kinds of thoughts in bed to bad sleepers.
People with insomnia are more focused on worries, problems, or noises in the environment, and are often preoccupied with not sleeping.
Good sleepers, on the other hand, typically have dream-like, hallucinatory, less ordered thoughts before nodding off.
Cognitive shuffling attempts to mimic the thinking patterns of good sleepers by simulating the dream-like and random thought patterns they generally have before drifting off to sleep.
In particular, Beaudoin's research describes two types of sleep-related thoughts: insomnolent (or anti-sleep) and pro-somnolent (sleep-promoting) thoughts.
Insomnolent thoughts include things such as worrying, planning, rehearsing, and ruminating on perceived problems or failings.
Pro-somnolent thoughts on the other hand involve thoughts that can help you fall asleep, such as dream-like imagery or having a calm, relaxed state of mind.
Cognitive shuffling aims to distract from or interfere with insomnolent thought. It offers a calm, neutral path for your racing mind, and can reduce the stress associated with not sleeping.
Cognitive shuffling also helps tell your brain you are ready for sleep.
In fact, the process of "shuffling" between different thoughts is similar to the way your brain naturally drifts off to sleep. During the transition to sleep, brain activity slows. Your brain starts to generate disconnected images and fleeting scenes, known as hypnagogic hallucinations, without a conscious effort to make sense of them.
By mimicking these scattered, disconnected, and random thought patterns, cognitive shuffling may help you transition from wakefulness to sleep.
And the preliminary research into this is promising. Beaudoin and his team have found serial diverse imagining helps to lower arousal before sleep, improve sleep quality and reduce the effort involved in falling asleep.
However, with only a small number of research studies, more work is needed here.
As with every new strategy, however, practise makes perfect. Don't be disheartened if you don't see an improvement straight away; these things take time.
Stay consistent and be kind to yourself.
And what works for some won't work for others. Different people benefit from different types of strategies depending on how they relate to and experience stress or stressful thoughts.
Other strategies to help create the right conditions for sleep include:
If, despite all your best efforts, night time thoughts continue to impact your sleep or overall wellbeing, consider seeking professional help from your doctor or a trained sleep specialist.
Melinda Jackson, Associate Professor at Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University and Eleni Kavaliotis, Research Fellow in the Sleep, Cognition, and Mood Laboratory at Monash University, Monash University
This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.
If you've been on social media lately - perhaps scrolling in the middle of the night, when you know you shouldn't but you just can't sleep - you might have seen those videos promoting a get-to-sleep technique called "cognitive shuffling".
The idea, proponents say, is to engage your mind with random ideas and images via a special formula:
It's popular on Instagram and TikTok, but does "cognitive shuffling" have any basis in science?
The cognitive shuffling technique was made famous by Canada-based researcher Luc P. Beaudoin more than a decade ago, when he published a paper about how what he called "serial diverse imagining" could help with sleep.
One of Beaudoin's hypothetical examples involved a woman thinking of the word "blanket", then thinking bicycle (and imagining a bicycle), buying (imagining buying shoes), banana (visualising a banana tree) and so on.
Soon, Beaudoin writes, she moves onto the letter L, thinking about her friend Larry, the word "like" (imagining her son hugging his dog). She soon transitions to the letter A, thinking of the word "Amsterdam":
"and she might very vaguely imagine the large hand of a sailor gesturing for another order of fries in an Amsterdam pub while a rancid accordion plays in the background."
Sleep soon ensues. The goal, according to Beaudoin, is to think briefly about:
"a neutral or pleasant target and frequently [switch] to unrelated targets (normally every 5-15 seconds)."
Don't try to relate one word with another or find a link between the words; resist the mind's natural tendency toward sense-making.
While the research into this technique is still in its infancy, the idea is grounded in science. That's because we know from other research good sleepers tend to have different kinds of thoughts in bed to bad sleepers.
People with insomnia are more focused on worries, problems, or noises in the environment, and are often preoccupied with not sleeping.
Good sleepers, on the other hand, typically have dream-like, hallucinatory, less ordered thoughts before nodding off.
Cognitive shuffling attempts to mimic the thinking patterns of good sleepers by simulating the dream-like and random thought patterns they generally have before drifting off to sleep.
In particular, Beaudoin's research describes two types of sleep-related thoughts: insomnolent (or anti-sleep) and pro-somnolent (sleep-promoting) thoughts.
Insomnolent thoughts include things such as worrying, planning, rehearsing, and ruminating on perceived problems or failings.
Pro-somnolent thoughts on the other hand involve thoughts that can help you fall asleep, such as dream-like imagery or having a calm, relaxed state of mind.
Cognitive shuffling aims to distract from or interfere with insomnolent thought. It offers a calm, neutral path for your racing mind, and can reduce the stress associated with not sleeping.
Cognitive shuffling also helps tell your brain you are ready for sleep.
In fact, the process of "shuffling" between different thoughts is similar to the way your brain naturally drifts off to sleep. During the transition to sleep, brain activity slows. Your brain starts to generate disconnected images and fleeting scenes, known as hypnagogic hallucinations, without a conscious effort to make sense of them.
By mimicking these scattered, disconnected, and random thought patterns, cognitive shuffling may help you transition from wakefulness to sleep.
And the preliminary research into this is promising. Beaudoin and his team have found serial diverse imagining helps to lower arousal before sleep, improve sleep quality and reduce the effort involved in falling asleep.
However, with only a small number of research studies, more work is needed here.
As with every new strategy, however, practise makes perfect. Don't be disheartened if you don't see an improvement straight away; these things take time.
Stay consistent and be kind to yourself.
And what works for some won't work for others. Different people benefit from different types of strategies depending on how they relate to and experience stress or stressful thoughts.
Other strategies to help create the right conditions for sleep include:
If, despite all your best efforts, night time thoughts continue to impact your sleep or overall wellbeing, consider seeking professional help from your doctor or a trained sleep specialist.
Melinda Jackson, Associate Professor at Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University and Eleni Kavaliotis, Research Fellow in the Sleep, Cognition, and Mood Laboratory at Monash University, Monash University
This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.
If you've been on social media lately - perhaps scrolling in the middle of the night, when you know you shouldn't but you just can't sleep - you might have seen those videos promoting a get-to-sleep technique called "cognitive shuffling".
The idea, proponents say, is to engage your mind with random ideas and images via a special formula:
It's popular on Instagram and TikTok, but does "cognitive shuffling" have any basis in science?
The cognitive shuffling technique was made famous by Canada-based researcher Luc P. Beaudoin more than a decade ago, when he published a paper about how what he called "serial diverse imagining" could help with sleep.
One of Beaudoin's hypothetical examples involved a woman thinking of the word "blanket", then thinking bicycle (and imagining a bicycle), buying (imagining buying shoes), banana (visualising a banana tree) and so on.
Soon, Beaudoin writes, she moves onto the letter L, thinking about her friend Larry, the word "like" (imagining her son hugging his dog). She soon transitions to the letter A, thinking of the word "Amsterdam":
"and she might very vaguely imagine the large hand of a sailor gesturing for another order of fries in an Amsterdam pub while a rancid accordion plays in the background."
Sleep soon ensues. The goal, according to Beaudoin, is to think briefly about:
"a neutral or pleasant target and frequently [switch] to unrelated targets (normally every 5-15 seconds)."
Don't try to relate one word with another or find a link between the words; resist the mind's natural tendency toward sense-making.
While the research into this technique is still in its infancy, the idea is grounded in science. That's because we know from other research good sleepers tend to have different kinds of thoughts in bed to bad sleepers.
People with insomnia are more focused on worries, problems, or noises in the environment, and are often preoccupied with not sleeping.
Good sleepers, on the other hand, typically have dream-like, hallucinatory, less ordered thoughts before nodding off.
Cognitive shuffling attempts to mimic the thinking patterns of good sleepers by simulating the dream-like and random thought patterns they generally have before drifting off to sleep.
In particular, Beaudoin's research describes two types of sleep-related thoughts: insomnolent (or anti-sleep) and pro-somnolent (sleep-promoting) thoughts.
Insomnolent thoughts include things such as worrying, planning, rehearsing, and ruminating on perceived problems or failings.
Pro-somnolent thoughts on the other hand involve thoughts that can help you fall asleep, such as dream-like imagery or having a calm, relaxed state of mind.
Cognitive shuffling aims to distract from or interfere with insomnolent thought. It offers a calm, neutral path for your racing mind, and can reduce the stress associated with not sleeping.
Cognitive shuffling also helps tell your brain you are ready for sleep.
In fact, the process of "shuffling" between different thoughts is similar to the way your brain naturally drifts off to sleep. During the transition to sleep, brain activity slows. Your brain starts to generate disconnected images and fleeting scenes, known as hypnagogic hallucinations, without a conscious effort to make sense of them.
By mimicking these scattered, disconnected, and random thought patterns, cognitive shuffling may help you transition from wakefulness to sleep.
And the preliminary research into this is promising. Beaudoin and his team have found serial diverse imagining helps to lower arousal before sleep, improve sleep quality and reduce the effort involved in falling asleep.
However, with only a small number of research studies, more work is needed here.
As with every new strategy, however, practise makes perfect. Don't be disheartened if you don't see an improvement straight away; these things take time.
Stay consistent and be kind to yourself.
And what works for some won't work for others. Different people benefit from different types of strategies depending on how they relate to and experience stress or stressful thoughts.
Other strategies to help create the right conditions for sleep include:
If, despite all your best efforts, night time thoughts continue to impact your sleep or overall wellbeing, consider seeking professional help from your doctor or a trained sleep specialist.
Melinda Jackson, Associate Professor at Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University and Eleni Kavaliotis, Research Fellow in the Sleep, Cognition, and Mood Laboratory at Monash University, Monash University
This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles

The Age
11 hours ago
- The Age
British parliament votes to end ‘cruel' abortion prosecutions for women
In 2023, Carla Foster received a 28-month prison sentence after she admitted taking abortion pills to induce a miscarriage during the initial COVID lockdown in 2020, when she was between 32 and 34 weeks pregnant. She was released after just one month in custody following a successful appeal. Another case involved Nicola Packer, who was charged after taking the medications mifepristone and misoprostol at about 26 weeks of pregnancy. She denied knowing that she was more than 10 weeks pregnant and was acquitted by a jury. The reform does not extend to medical professionals or anyone else involved in abortions performed outside existing legal frameworks. Nor does it alter the clinical requirements of the 1967 Act, which permits abortions up to 24 weeks with authorisation from two doctors, and after that point only under limited and exceptional circumstances. Not all within the government backed the change. Justice Secretary Shabana Mahmood opposed the amendment, warning of risks it might introduce. 'I am deeply concerned to see these measures being progressed in the name of women's rights, when the potential physical and mental impacts on women would be so devastating,' she wrote to constituents. 'I oppose extending abortions up until the point of birth beyond the exemptions that currently exist, as doing so would not only be unnecessary but dangerous.' Antoniazzi made clear the measure would not affect healthcare provisions: the clause 'would not change any law regarding the provision of abortion services within a healthcare setting', she said, reiterating that the current time limits and clinical conditions would remain. The UK vote lands at a moment of flux in abortion access across Europe, where legal protections are often undermined by logistical or cultural barriers. Despite most EU countries allowing abortion under certain conditions, many women — an estimated 4500 in 2022 — still travel abroad to access care. Loading The Netherlands received 2762 women from Germany, Poland, Belgium, France, and Ireland in 2022 alone. Spain recorded about 1500 cross-border abortion cases in the same year. Hundreds more travelled to the UK for the same reason. Notably, only two EU member states – Malta and Poland – impose near-total bans. But restrictive time limits, a lack of providers and religious or cultural resistance elsewhere mean that abortion, while legal on paper, is inaccessible in practice. Women in France, Italy, and Croatia, for example, often struggle to access care due to 'conscientious objectors' – doctors who refuse to perform abortions. In some Italian regions, up to 90 per cent of physicians decline to carry out the procedure. Even in France, one-third of the UK-based Abortion Support Network's clients are French, many from rural areas without clinics. A recent survey found the average delay between discovering a pregnancy and accessing an abortion was four weeks – often because of waiting periods, lack of information, or difficulty in securing funds and time off work. A citizen-led initiative titled My Voice, My Choice has gathered more than a million signatures calling on the EU to fund abortion access across borders. But progress has been slow. The EU's health commissioner said the bloc 'stands ready to support member states,' but reiterated that abortion remains a matter of national jurisdiction.


West Australian
13 hours ago
- West Australian
WA Government awards Adelaide company nearly $400,000 to combat European Starlings with AI
The WA Government is turning to artificial intelligence to scare off European starlings from fruit crops and livestock feed along the south coast, awarding an Adelaide company a nearly $400,000 tender. The project will hinge on devices capable of sensing the bird, classed as a pest in Australia, from up to 100m away and will aim to deploy 20 portable, automated acoustic surveillance devices throughout high-risk habitats along about 700kms of WA's south coast. The $373,230 tender was awarded to Adelaide-based company Supersensory Technologies earlier this month. About 400kms west of the South Australian border is covered by the project as well as about 300kms between Cape Arid and Hopetoun. The AI-geared devices will be installed as part of a Department of Primary Industry and Regional Development trapping program for the avian pests that has run since the 1970s. DPIRD senior research scientist Dr Susan Campbell said the automated acoustic surveillance devices would help bolster WA's biosecurity defences against European starlings. 'Scaling up our automated field response with these new devices increases our surveillance footprint both spatially and temporally, enabling DPIRD to mount a rapid response to any verified starling incursion,' she said. 'Commencing in the 1970s, a near continuous early detection and rapid response management program has ensured that WA remains free from established starling populations. 'However, because starlings are firmly established throughout eastern Australia, incursions represent an ongoing risk to WA's agriculture, environment and socio-cultural assets.' The project is designed to prevent the encroachment of the birds from eastern Australia, after a breeding pair were located and removed from an Esperance house late last year. It marked the first detection on the south coast in more than a decade, and sparked a rapid biosecurity response. European starlings are a 'highly adaptable' bird, according to Dr Campbell, that can disperse over large distances and rapidly breed under good conditions. Starlings can impact horticulture, grain, and livestock industries — damaging fruit crops, like cherries and grapes, and consuming high volumes of livestock feed. Dr Campbell said the devices would have the ability to constantly capture and process acoustic information through a starling-specific 'machine learning algorithm'. She said the project would help to 'scale up' the department's ongoing surveillance of the pest. 'The devices then remotely communicate plausible starling detections for manual verification in close to 'real-time',' she said.


Perth Now
15 hours ago
- Perth Now
Mum forced to defend decision to share photo of deceased son
WARNING: SENSITIVE CONTENT An Australian influencer has been forced to defend her decision to share a photo of her deceased baby online after receiving backlash from her followers. Veruca Salt posted the picture of her late baby son, Cash, as her lay embalmed in his coffin to her Instagram stories on Tuesday. The post sparked heated debate online, with some defending Veruca's decision, while others found the photo to be too confronting to be shared to the internet. The Gold Coast influencer has since spoken her truth and addressed the torrent of hate she received in the wake of the post. 'Ya'll come onto a grieving mothers Instagram and be shocked when you see that there is actually a dead baby. Genuine question, are you stupid? Are you slow?' she said in a video posted to Instagram stories. 'You want me to explain in detail the horrible things that have happened. How did he die? What happened with your baby daddy? . . . .But then you bitch and moan when you actually see it. 'He's embalmed in a coffin and he looks beautiful, so you can suck it.' Many of the people who spoke out against Veruca took issue with the photo because they claim they were not presented with a content warning before the image appeared on their screen. A sensitive content warning has since been added to the photo, warning users the image may be upsetting. Veruca's post addressing the backlash. Credit: Instagram In a follow-up Instagram story, Veruca wrote an emotional response to air her experience as a grieving mother. 'I'm triggered every time I hear a mum complain about her baby being sick, pretending her kid having a cold is similar to them dying,' she wrote. 'I'm triggered at every pregnancy announcement, every birth video, every video of an older sibling meeting the new baby at the hospital, every video of a baby with their mum. 'My son's death isn't about you.' Cash was just six weeks old when he tragically died of sudden infant death syndrome in February last year. PerthNow has contacted Veruca for comment. Lifeline: 13 11 14.